全文获取类型
收费全文 | 19922篇 |
免费 | 915篇 |
国内免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
20855篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 159篇 |
2020年 | 264篇 |
2019年 | 300篇 |
2018年 | 436篇 |
2017年 | 428篇 |
2016年 | 466篇 |
2015年 | 333篇 |
2014年 | 404篇 |
2013年 | 1676篇 |
2012年 | 718篇 |
2011年 | 783篇 |
2010年 | 460篇 |
2009年 | 416篇 |
2008年 | 589篇 |
2007年 | 600篇 |
2006年 | 517篇 |
2005年 | 469篇 |
2004年 | 458篇 |
2003年 | 434篇 |
2002年 | 423篇 |
2001年 | 749篇 |
2000年 | 743篇 |
1999年 | 502篇 |
1998年 | 240篇 |
1997年 | 225篇 |
1996年 | 173篇 |
1994年 | 161篇 |
1993年 | 176篇 |
1992年 | 393篇 |
1991年 | 346篇 |
1990年 | 365篇 |
1989年 | 362篇 |
1988年 | 351篇 |
1987年 | 313篇 |
1986年 | 325篇 |
1985年 | 323篇 |
1984年 | 272篇 |
1983年 | 241篇 |
1982年 | 163篇 |
1979年 | 215篇 |
1978年 | 197篇 |
1975年 | 207篇 |
1974年 | 263篇 |
1973年 | 217篇 |
1972年 | 209篇 |
1971年 | 171篇 |
1969年 | 200篇 |
1968年 | 202篇 |
1967年 | 165篇 |
1966年 | 183篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
M H Kelly J K Bock 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(3):389-403
The goals of this research were to determine whether speakers adjust the stress patterns of words within sentences to create an alternation between strong and weak beats and to explore whether this rhythmic alternation contributes to the characteristics stress differences between two major lexical categories of English. Two experiments suggested that speakers do alter lexical stress in accordance with rhythmic biases. When speakers produced disyllabic pseudowords in sentence contexts, they were more likely to place stress on the first syllable when the pseudoword was preceded by a weak stress and followed by a strong one than when the strong stress preceded and the weak followed. This occurred both when the pseudowords served as nouns and when they served as verbs. Text analyses further revealed that weakly stressed elements precede nouns more often than verbs, whereas such elements follow verbs more often than nouns. Thus, disyllabic nouns are more likely than disyllabic verbs to occupy contexts biased toward trochaic rhythm, a finding consistent with leftward dominant stress in disyllabic English nouns. The history of stress changes in English nouns and verbs also conforms with the view that rhythmic context may have contributed to the evolution of stress differences. Together, the findings suggest that the citation stress patterns of words may to some degree reflect adaptations of lexical knowledge to conditions of language performance. 相似文献
55.
For 3 consecutive days cockroaches (Periplaneta americana) were exposed to either escapable, inescapable, or no shock in an escape task. Twenty-four hours later they were tested in a shuttlebox escape task. There were reliable differences between escapable and inescapable animals and between inescapable and control animals in both escape latencies and the number of failures to escape. 相似文献
56.
Ceci and Liker (1986b) presented data that they contended shows two things: (a) Handicapping harness races is a cognitively complex undertaking that can be captured by a multiple regression model, and (b) neither overall skill at handicapping nor the complexity of the mental model used is related to standard measures of intelligence. The first contention is not at issue. But the second contention, that handicapping performance is unrelated to IQ, is not supported by the data presented; in fact, the opposite conclusion seems more likely. The purpose of this comment is to point out errors frequently made in individual-differences research concerning population definition, sample selection, dependent and independent variable reliability and validity and interpretation of results. 相似文献
57.
Summary The interactive-activation model postulates (a) that activation at the letter level leads automatically to activation at the word level, (b) that the word-superiority effect reflects reactivation of letters by the word they spell, and (c) that subjects identify words on the basis of information obtained from separate letter-position channels. In the first two experiments, we showed words in upper, lower, or mixed case: the word-superiority effect was reduced when words were presented in mixed-case letters, presumably because extra-letter information is lost with mixed-case presentation; i.e., postulate (c) is wrong. The third experiment showed that when the letters of a word are rotated 180° subjects can identify the letters without producing a word-superiority effect; i.e., one of postulates (a) and (b) is wrong. In Experiments 4 and 5, we trained subjects to name words presented in inverted letters; training was more effective when subjects could exploit bigram information in addition to letter-channel information; i.e., reading inverted text is based on extra-letter-feature information, not on a general skill in rotating letters. Taken together, our data deny three of the interactive-activation model's major postulates. We offer some suggestions for future versions of the model.
Electronic mail: Userid: MEWHORTD; Nodeid: QUCDN; Domain: BITNET 相似文献
58.
K J Arbour D M Wilkie 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1988,102(2):177-181
When given a choice between two mashes of equal caloric density but differing flavors, rats (Rattus norvegicus) show a robust preference for the flavor previously associated with a higher calorie food. This finding suggests that rats may identify food quality by sensory cues such as taste. Our initial attempt to show this effect in the golden Syrian hamster (Mesocricetus auratus) failed, apparently because of this species's tendency to store both high- and low-calorie mashes in their cheek pouches during conditioning trials. Initially we attempted to circumvent this seeming morphological constraint on learning by presenting low- and high-calorie mashes on alternate days. This procedure too failed to produce evidence of flavor-caloric learning, although this procedure produced robust learning in another rodent, the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Another method of preventing cheek pouching--concurrently presenting low- and high-calorie liquid diets--was more successful; then, hamsters showed clear evidence of flavor-caloric learning. Thus, although flavor-caloric learning is demonstrable in species of rodent besides the rat, the circumstances under which it occurs vary. 相似文献
59.
It is proposed that expert knowledge can operate as a cognitive cueing structure for the acquisition and retention of new information in memory. Two experiments are reported which demonstrate that expert knowledge about football and clothing can act as mnemonic cues for the recall of information newly associated with that knowledge. In Experiment 1 expert terms from the domains of football and clothing and those neutral nouns paired with them were both better recalled by experts than by non-experts. In Experiment 2 passages containing information contrary to factual knowledge about football and clothing were recalled better by experts than by non-experts, in spite of the fact that information in the passages contradicted what the experts already knew. The results of the two experiments were interpreted as showing that expert knowledge provides mental cues that have desirable mnemonic properties such as constructibility, associability, discriminability and invertibility. Also, the interpretation of expert knowledge as a cognitive cueing structure is compared to Ausubel's ideas regarding advance organizers. 相似文献
60.
In this study, the relations among depression, anxiety, and neuroticism measured by self-report questionnaires were investigated. Subjects were 207 psychiatric patients. High correlations were found among self-report scales purporting to measure depression, anxiety, and neuroticism. Results of a content analysis showed considerable overlap among these scales. A division of items into six content categories did not result in lower correlations compared to the original scales. 相似文献