全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4255篇 |
免费 | 50篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
4306篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 21篇 |
2021年 | 34篇 |
2020年 | 55篇 |
2019年 | 86篇 |
2018年 | 110篇 |
2017年 | 108篇 |
2016年 | 135篇 |
2015年 | 76篇 |
2014年 | 129篇 |
2013年 | 469篇 |
2012年 | 185篇 |
2011年 | 195篇 |
2010年 | 113篇 |
2009年 | 96篇 |
2008年 | 153篇 |
2007年 | 187篇 |
2006年 | 153篇 |
2005年 | 167篇 |
2004年 | 154篇 |
2003年 | 145篇 |
2002年 | 131篇 |
2001年 | 70篇 |
2000年 | 67篇 |
1999年 | 57篇 |
1998年 | 69篇 |
1997年 | 66篇 |
1996年 | 43篇 |
1995年 | 39篇 |
1994年 | 56篇 |
1993年 | 46篇 |
1992年 | 48篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 39篇 |
1988年 | 44篇 |
1987年 | 38篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 36篇 |
1984年 | 51篇 |
1983年 | 51篇 |
1982年 | 48篇 |
1981年 | 40篇 |
1980年 | 30篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 46篇 |
1977年 | 33篇 |
1976年 | 41篇 |
1975年 | 28篇 |
1974年 | 34篇 |
1973年 | 30篇 |
排序方式: 共有4306条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
162.
While self-disclosure plays a prominent role in the development and maintenance of the therapeutic relationship, most marriage and family therapy clinical research fails to include measures of self-disclosure. A factor analysis of Chelune's (1976) Self Disclosure Situations Survey (SDSS) reduces the number of items in this measure from 20 to six. Researchers are encouraged to consider conducting clinical research with specific attention to the construct of self-disclosure. The revised SDSS could easily be added as part of the self-report information collected in clinical studies. 相似文献
163.
Belva C. Collins Meada Hall Terri A. Branson Michael Holder 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1999,9(3-4):223-237
Two secondary students with moderate disabilities (one per classroom) attended Advanced English classes with peers without disabilities in a rural high school. In additional to conducting planned daily instruction, each English teacher also systematically presented three sets of information (two facts per set) to each student with disabilities during the course of the class using a parallel treatments design. Each set of information included a fact related to the English class (e.g., Begin each sentence with a capital letter.) and a fact not related to the English class (e.g., The governor of Kentucky is Paul Patton.). The special education teacher conducted daily probe sessions to document acquisition. Of the six facts presented to each student with disabilities, one student acquired two related facts and one unrelated fact, while the other student acquired two related and two unrelated facts. This investigation implies that students who are fully included can acquire information presented by the regular classroom teacher during the course of a typical class and that teachers can facilitate learning by planning to present such information in a systematic fashion. 相似文献
164.
The semantic, or model‐theoretic, approach to theories has recently come under criticism on two fronts: (i) it is claimed that it cannot account for the wide diversity of models employed in scientific practice—a claim which has led some to propose a “deflationary” account of models; (ii) it is further contended that the sense of “model” used by the approach differs from that given in model theory. Our aim in the present work is to articulate a possible response to these claims, drawing on recent developments within the semantic approach itself. Thus, the first is answered by utilizing the notion of a “partial structure”, first introduced in this context by da Costa and French in 1990. The second claim is undermined by consideration of van Fraassen's understanding of “model” which corresponds well with that evinced by modem mathematicians. This latter discussion, in particular, has an impact on the continuing debate regarding the relative merits of the semantic and syntactic views and the developments presented here can be taken to provide further support to the former. 相似文献
165.
Huprich SK 《Journal of personality disorders》2005,19(6):659-673
The purpose of this study was to test a priori predictions about the way in which avoidant personality disorder (APD) can be differentiated from depressive personality disorder (DPD) in a clinical population. Psychiatric outpatients were administered two measures of DPD, including the SCID-II for other DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders, along with criterion measures upon which the two disorders would be differentiated. APD was found to be most strongly associated with state and trait measures of anxiety, while DPD was most strongly associated with state and trait measures of hostility. Individuals with DPD had higher mean scores on measures of hostility than those without DPD, and individuals with APD had higher mean scores on measures of anxiety than those without APD. However, DPD measures were also significantly correlated with state and trait measures of anxiety and APD with measures of depressive symptoms. Furthermore, anxiety was found to be higher in some groups of individuals with DPD than those with APD. It is concluded that the level of hostility in this DPD population appears to be an important symptom by which to differentiate the two disorders and that a reconsideration of including DPD criterion #4 -- prone to brooding and worrying -- may be justified. Furthermore, the SCID-II interview may be better at differentiating DPD and APD than a self-report measure of DPD. 相似文献
166.
167.
168.
Martinus J. A. Buekers Richard A. Magill Kellie G. Hall 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1992,44(1):105-117
Even though it can be shown that verbal knowledge of results (KR) is redundant with sensory feedback for learning certain motor skills, such findings do not eliminate the possibility that when KR is available it influences underlying learning processes. In order to examine the function of KR more closely, two experiments were designed in which the subjects received conflicting information about their own sensory feedback and the KR presented by the experimenter. In Experiment 1, two erroneous-KR groups, a correct-KR group, and a no-KR group performed 150 practice trials on a simple anticipation timing task and then performed three no-KR retention tests of 30 trials each following intervals of 10 minutes, 1 week, and 1 month. The results supported previous findings that providing correct KR is redundant in anticipation tasks. However, learning was influenced by KR as subjects performed according to the erroneous KR information, thereby ignoring their sensory feedback even after a 1-month interval. In Experiment 2, subjects practised a more complex striking response for the anticipation task for 75 trials and then performed no-KR retention trials either immediately, or 1 day or 1 week later. One of the groups received erroneous KR after 50 practice trials with correct KR. The results confirmed and extended those from Experiment 1, as erroneous KR, even after initial practice with correct KR, influenced retention performance. These results indicate that although KR provides information that is not needed to learn anticipation timing skills, this augmented verbal information is a dominant source of information that influences underlying cognitive processes involved in learning motor skills. 相似文献
169.
170.