全文获取类型
收费全文 | 62128篇 |
免费 | 914篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
63048篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 404篇 |
2019年 | 476篇 |
2018年 | 3945篇 |
2017年 | 3288篇 |
2016年 | 2864篇 |
2015年 | 736篇 |
2014年 | 728篇 |
2013年 | 3031篇 |
2012年 | 1748篇 |
2011年 | 3446篇 |
2010年 | 3138篇 |
2009年 | 2101篇 |
2008年 | 2647篇 |
2007年 | 3129篇 |
2006年 | 952篇 |
2005年 | 1119篇 |
2004年 | 1002篇 |
2003年 | 885篇 |
2002年 | 854篇 |
2001年 | 1349篇 |
2000年 | 1316篇 |
1999年 | 943篇 |
1998年 | 479篇 |
1997年 | 413篇 |
1992年 | 897篇 |
1991年 | 834篇 |
1990年 | 845篇 |
1989年 | 748篇 |
1988年 | 752篇 |
1987年 | 712篇 |
1986年 | 734篇 |
1985年 | 818篇 |
1984年 | 632篇 |
1983年 | 593篇 |
1982年 | 403篇 |
1981年 | 392篇 |
1979年 | 714篇 |
1978年 | 463篇 |
1977年 | 435篇 |
1976年 | 441篇 |
1975年 | 602篇 |
1974年 | 693篇 |
1973年 | 737篇 |
1972年 | 626篇 |
1971年 | 588篇 |
1970年 | 581篇 |
1969年 | 568篇 |
1968年 | 755篇 |
1967年 | 667篇 |
1966年 | 599篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
G E Kühne R Beier M Heim W Knorr L Perina H Richter V Rossberg R Sorger N Waldow 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1979,31(4):203-212
The antidepressants trimipramine and imipramine were compared within the framework of a multiclinical study performed under the conditions of a controlled clinical experiment. There has been found a time-different remission of affective und psychomotoric symptoms. The panthymoleptic action of trimipramine and other antidepressants is discussed with reference to these results. Trimipramine influences psychotic states, especially if in depression anxiety is combined with agitation, also in hypochondriac forms of depression. 相似文献
992.
Self-selected paraprofessional trainees enrolled in a helping skills training program participated in a 6-minute pretraining helping interview from which their frequency of continuing responses was obtained. Continuing responses allow helpees to present their concerns in a nonthreatening, supportive environment and are important in the relationship establishing stage of the helping process. For data analysis purposes only, trainees were divided into three groups, a high, medium, and low group, based on the frequency of continuing responses made. Following training, at posttest, no significant differences were found among the three groups on the amount of continuing responses made during a second identical 6-minute interview. Implications for various selection procedures and training are discussed. 相似文献
993.
H Goodglass S E Blumstein J B Gleason M R Hyde E Green S Statlender 《Brain and language》1979,7(2):201-209
This study compared two levels of syntactic encoding with respect to their effect on aphasics' auditory comprehension. The same informational content was presented either as an embedded sentence or as two contiguous simplé propositions. A similar contrast was utilized to compare sentences containing relational terms of time, comparison, instrumentality, as well as sentences involving conjunction reduction, with parallel messages expressed in expanded, simplified form. A picture-verification paradigm was used. Comprehension was significantly enhanced only in the case of embedded sentences, with a tendency for Broca's aphasics to show the maximum effects. 相似文献
994.
R W Proctor J D Proctor 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1979,5(4):610-624
Previous studies have obtained conflicting evidence regarding the effect of probe modality when a secondary probe task is used to measure attentional demands of a primary letter-matching task. The present experiments found no difference in the shape of the probe reaction time functions for auditory and visual probes as long as probe modality was known. When probes "unexpectedly" occurred in the modality that was different from the majority of the probes, differences were obtained. Only the unexpected auditory function changed, shape relative to the others, with reaction time to probes that occurred early in the matching task sequence being increased. The results are interpreted as supporting a role of attentional capacity in the activation of structures that accept relevant sensory input. 相似文献
995.
Three experiments investigated the integrality of height and width of rectangles and the ability of observers to selectively attend to only one dimension. In Experiment 1, redundancy gain and orthogonal interference were demonstrated in a same/different task. Orthogonal interference was due to the output of the "irrelevant" analyzer interfering with the output of the "relevant" analyzer. These results indicated that height and width are integral, but they can be most parsimoniously explained by assuming that rectangles are initially processed by separate dimensional analyzers. Experiment I demonstrated that with sufficient practice (160 trials), observers are able to selectively attend to the more frequently relevant dimension. Performance for the stressed dimension increased, whereas performance for the unstressed dimension declined. Experiment 3 also demonstrated that with sufficient practice (192 trials), observers are able to selectively attend to the relevant dimension and ignore the irrelevant dimension. Orthogonal interference disappeared. The results are discussed in terms of the ability of observers to modify the perceptual process. 相似文献
996.
Summary In an attempt to discover patterns of adult figurative language use, preference and comprehension, 30 Ss were asked to complete 4 different tasks: a composition, a test of simile preference, a test of simile production, and a test of metaphoric comprehension. Each test provided scores for both novel and clichéed usage. The composition task was also scored for total words while preference and production tasks were scored for literal usage, thus producing a total of 11 dependent variables across the complete set of tasks.Results revealed that Ss selected a higher number of novel figures under choice relative to production conditions. Correlational results revealed relationships across all 4 tasks for the categories of novel and frozen usage although certain tasks (i.e., simile preference and production) were more sensitive to novel diction while others (comprehension) were more sensitive to frozen diction. Within-task correlations, on the other hand, showed unique patterns for each of the tasks. Factor analyses of these correlations were found to produce a series of consistent three-factor solutions with the first of these factors concerned primarily with novel diction, the second with both novel and frozen comprehension, and the third primarily with frozen diction.Taken in combination, present results were interpreted to mean that while the distinction between novel and frozen is a meaningful one, specific task requirements can and do moderate the pattern of relationships obtained. This suggests that figurative language competence may not be a unitary process but one that is strongly affected by specific task and situational constraints and that future work, both methodological and theoretical, must take these factors into account. 相似文献
997.
Steven Van Auken 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1979,58(2):143-144
Counselors of unruly, runaway, truant, or delinquent children and their families run the risk of emotional overexposure– “burnout” as a result of intense client needs and problems. Survival on the job requires taking steps to avoid internalizing caseload pressures. These steps include: helping-not replacing-parents, keeping meetings brief, responding to emergencies selectively, taking simple steps first, using humor, taking care of oneself, expecting complications, avoiding panic, and knowing when to withdraw. 相似文献
998.
Head movement can have a significant effect on the ability to locate the direction of a sound source. A system has been designed to track the head movement in response to sound originating at different azimuth locations with respect to the head. A videotape record is made of a light approximating a point source carried on a lightweight “beanie” mounted on the listener’s head. Movement of the light is monitored by the video camera and recorded on tape, along with the sound stimulus and information concerning loudspeaker location and time. The horizontal and vertical coordinates of the light-spot image are determined in relation to the video synch pulses defining the field borders. Synch signals are available from a video monitor either in real-time or from tape replay to define each TV frame and horizontal scan line. The circuitry interfaces to a computer programmed to take the information, apply a calibration, and process the data into records of time-varying head position and velocity. Examples of both digital and graphic printouts of head movement are given. The system is capable of expansion to three-axis operation. 相似文献
999.
The KIM-1 microprocessor combined with a plug-board interface provides an inexpensive and highly flexible system for controlling classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. The system is described in the context of a program for differential conditioning. 相似文献
1000.
A microprocessor system can be used both to control aversive conditioning (classical, signaled and Sidman avoidance, and conflict) experiments and to acquire behavioral data in multiple subjects at a fraction of the cost of solid state systems. The software described in this paper can be used to program tones and shocks and count responses, independently, in up to 12 animals simultaneously. 相似文献