首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   864篇
  免费   37篇
  2023年   8篇
  2021年   11篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   13篇
  2018年   27篇
  2017年   27篇
  2016年   27篇
  2015年   22篇
  2014年   30篇
  2013年   109篇
  2012年   41篇
  2011年   44篇
  2010年   26篇
  2009年   28篇
  2008年   27篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   34篇
  2004年   30篇
  2003年   29篇
  2002年   35篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   16篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   19篇
  1996年   9篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   14篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   9篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1973年   6篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   3篇
  1967年   3篇
排序方式: 共有901条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
841.
842.
This article focuses on the political participation of ministers from five evangelical Protestant denominations that differ in theology, polity, and history. Despite such differences, these clergy respond to political influences in much the same fashion. We find that the standard theories of political participation have varying success in accounting for their political involvement. Sociodemographic explanations provide little help, but psychological engagement with politics has more explanatory power. Professional role orientations are the best predictors of actual participation. And the clergy who see moral reform issues as the most important confronting the country—and who hold conservative views on such issues—are most likely to become engaged. Finally, membership in Christian Right organizations serves to elicit more activity than might occur if ministers were left to internally motivated participation. Despite the emphasis on other contextual variables in some work on clerical politics, we find that communications exposures, congregational influences, and even the support of clerical colleagues have very limited independent effects on political involvement.  相似文献   
843.
844.
Abstract. This essay explores the terrain of technology in theological education and offers a typology for how technology is used in seminary contexts. The author surveys 43 seminaries in North America to gain insight into the attitudes of faculty toward the use of technology in their teaching and for use in the preparation of ministers. Reflections on the typology in the concluding section offer fuel for subsequent work on the topic.  相似文献   
845.
846.
847.
848.
Thirty subjects participated in a study of the test-retest reliability of five psychophysiological assessment procedures: habituation. Quiz Electrocardiogram, anger imagery, aversive slides and startle. The procedures were presented in identical fashion, 2 weeks apart. Skin conductance level and response, heart rate, vasomotor response, skin temperature, frontalis and forearm electromyogram, respiration rate. and systolic and diastolic blood pressure were recorded continuously. The following analyses were performed on Test-Retest data: correlations, baseline changes, profile similarity, and stability of ranked standardized physiological scores. Significant but modest Test-Retest correlations were found for skin conductance level and response, skin temperature, respiration rate, heart rate and systolic blood pressure. Absolute scores were more often stable than change scores. All baselines were stable from Test to Retest. Four baselines and four test procedures had Test-Retest Profile Similarity Index scores indicating reliability. Chi-square analyses of Retest ranks of standardized physiological scores showed that the two most and least responsive measures accounted for Test-Retest stability. Conclusions were: (a) Psychophysiological response patterns are reliable for baselines and procedures, and single psychophysiological measures (especially absolute scores) are moderately stable; (b) Traditional (correlational) estimates of the reliability of single psychophysiological responses are limited by individual-response stereotypy, but multivariate analysis of response pattern provides a better index of reliability.  相似文献   
849.
We present a theory of conversation comprehension in which a line of the conversation is “understood” by relating it to one of seven possible “points”. We define these points, and present examples where it seems plausible that the failure to “get the point” would indeed constitute a failure to understand the conversation. We argue that the recognition of such points must proceed in both a top down and bottom up fashion, and thus is likely to be quite complicated. Finally, we see the processing of information in the conversation to be dependent upon which point classification the user decides upon.  相似文献   
850.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号