全文获取类型
收费全文 | 704篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
719篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 94篇 |
2012年 | 35篇 |
2011年 | 35篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 21篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有719条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Steve Lynch William A Watts Charles Galloway Spyros Tryphonopoulos 《Journal of research in personality》1973,7(1):71-77
Anxiety was induced in first/only born and later born subjects by the threatened injection of a harmless drug. Perceived appropriateness of the induced anxiety was manipulated by the experimenter's suggestion that anxiety over an injection was either perfectly natural (appropriate condition) or to be found only in rather nervous and effeminate men (inappropriate condition). The results indicated that the anxiety induction was successful, but that the perceived appropriateness of the anxiety determined the tendency to affiliate. Subjects in the appropriate condition showed a significantly greater tendency to affiliate than did subjects in the inappropriate condition. Birth order was related to anxiety on one of two measures, but was not implicated in the determination of the affiliative tendency. 相似文献
132.
133.
Although most theories of choice are cognitive, recent research has emphasized the role of emotions. We used a novel context--the Mad Cow crisis in France--to investigate how emotions alter choice even when consequences are held constant. A field study showed that individuals reduced beef consumption in months after many newspaper articles featured the emotional label "Mad Cow," but beef consumption was unaffected after articles featured scientific labels for the same disease. The reverse pattern held for the disease-related actions of a government bureaucracy. A lab study showed that the Mad Cow label induces people to make choices based solely on emotional reactions, whereas scientific labels induce people to consider their own probability judgments. Although the Mad Cow label produces less rational behavior than scientific labels, it is two to four times more common in the environment. 相似文献
134.
George J. DuPaul Paul L. Morgan George Farkas Marianne M. Hillemeier Steve Maczuga 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2018,46(5):979-992
We examined trajectories of academic and social functioning in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to identify those who might be at risk for especially severe levels of academic and social impairment over time. We estimated a series of growth mixture models using data from two subsamples of children participating in the NIMH Collaborative Multisite Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) including those with at least baseline and 96-month data for reading and mathematics achievement (n = 392; 77.3% male; M age = 7.7; SD = 0.8) or social skills ratings from teachers (n = 259; 74.9% male; M age = 7.6; SD = 0.8). We compared latent trajectories for children with ADHD to mean observed trajectories obtained from a local normative (i.e., non-ADHD) comparison group (n = 289; 80.6% male; M age = 9.9; SD = 1.1). Results indicated six latent trajectory classes for reading and mathematics and four classes for teacher social skills ratings. There was not only a relationship between trajectories of inattention symptoms and academic impairment, but also a similarly strong association between trajectory classes of hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and achievement. Trajectory class membership correlated with socio-demographic and diagnostic characteristics, inattention and hyperactive-impulsive symptom trajectories, externalizing behavior in school, and treatment receipt and dosage. Although children with ADHD display substantial heterogeneity in their reading, math, and social skills growth trajectories, those with behavioral and socio-demographic disadvantages are especially likely to display severe levels of academic and social impairment over time. Evidence-based early screening and intervention that directly address academic and social impairments in elementary school-aged children with ADHD are warranted. The ClinicalTrials.gov identifier is NCT00000388. 相似文献
135.
The object of my aggression: Sexual objectification increases physical aggression toward women
下载免费PDF全文

136.
137.
Roger C. Schank Gregg C. Collins Ernest Davis Peter N. Johnson Steve Lytinen Brian J. Reiser 《Cognitive Science》1982,6(3):255-275
We present a theory of conversation comprehension in which a line of the conversation is “understood” by relating it to one of seven possible “points”. We define these points, and present examples where it seems plausible that the failure to “get the point” would indeed constitute a failure to understand the conversation. We argue that the recognition of such points must proceed in both a top down and bottom up fashion, and thus is likely to be quite complicated. Finally, we see the processing of information in the conversation to be dependent upon which point classification the user decides upon. 相似文献
138.
Self-control in mentally retarded adolescents: choice as a function of amount and delay of reinforcement.
下载免费PDF全文

Three severely mentally retarded adolescents were studied under discrete-trial procedures in which a choice was arranged between edible reinforcers that differed in magnitude and, in some conditions, delay. In the absence of delays the larger reinforcer was consistently chosen. Under conditions in which the smaller reinforcer was not delayed, increasing the delay to delivery of the larger reinforcer decreased the percentage of trials in which that reinforcer was chosen. All subjects directed the majority of choice responses to the smaller reinforcer when the larger reinforcer was sufficiently delayed, although the value at which this occurred differed across subjects. Under conditions in which the larger reinforcer initially was sufficiently delayed to result in preference for the smaller one, progressively increasing in 5-s increments the delay to both reinforcers increased percentage of trials with the larger reinforcer chosen. At sufficiently long delays, 2 of the subjects consistently chose the larger, but more delayed, reinforcer, and the 3rd subject chose that reinforcer on half of the trials. These results are consistent with the findings of prior studies in which adult humans responded to terminate noise and pigeons responded to produce food. 相似文献
139.
To estimate the prevalence of being well-adjusted in adolescence, boys and girls with (n = 96) and without (n = 126) attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were assessed seven times in eight years starting when they were 4–6 years
of age. Symptoms of ADHD, ODD/CD, and depression/anxiety in addition to social skills and social preference were gathered
using multiple methods and informants. Being well-adjusted was defined by surpassing thresholds in at least four of the five
domains. At the 7- and 8-year follow-up, when youth were 11–14 years old, probands were significantly less likely to be well-adjusted
relative to age- and ethnicity-matched control children. Only a minority of children with ADHD was well-adjusted in adolescence
when emotional, behavioral, and social domains were considered simultaneously. Even when their ADHD symptoms improved over
time, most probands exhibited significant impairment 7–8 years after their initial assessment. 相似文献
140.
Massetti GM Lahey BB Pelham WE Loney J Ehrhardt A Lee SS Kipp H 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2008,36(3):399-410
The predictive validity of symptom criteria for different subtypes of ADHD among children who were impaired in at least one
setting in early childhood was examined. Academic achievement was assessed seven times over 8 years in 125 children who met
symptom criteria for ADHD at 4–6 years of age and in 130 demographically-matched non-referred comparison children. When intelligence
and other confounds were controlled, children who met modified criteria for the predominantly inattentive subtype of ADHD
in wave 1 had lower reading, spelling, and mathematics scores over time than both comparison children and children who met
modified criteria for the other subtypes of ADHD. In some analyses, children who met modified criteria for the combined type
had somewhat lower mathematics scores than comparison children. The robust academic deficits relative to intelligence in the
inattentive group in this age range suggest either that inattention results in academic underachievement or that some children
in the inattentive group have learning disabilities that cause secondary symptoms of inattention. Unexpectedly, wave 1 internalizing
(anxiety and depression) symptoms independently predicted deficits in academic achievement controlling ADHD, intelligence,
and other predictors. 相似文献