全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4333篇 |
免费 | 179篇 |
专业分类
4512篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 66篇 |
2019年 | 56篇 |
2018年 | 98篇 |
2017年 | 110篇 |
2016年 | 160篇 |
2015年 | 74篇 |
2014年 | 120篇 |
2013年 | 560篇 |
2012年 | 170篇 |
2011年 | 189篇 |
2010年 | 100篇 |
2009年 | 146篇 |
2008年 | 178篇 |
2007年 | 180篇 |
2006年 | 193篇 |
2005年 | 118篇 |
2004年 | 160篇 |
2003年 | 143篇 |
2002年 | 140篇 |
2001年 | 72篇 |
2000年 | 65篇 |
1999年 | 66篇 |
1998年 | 73篇 |
1997年 | 72篇 |
1996年 | 60篇 |
1995年 | 54篇 |
1994年 | 72篇 |
1993年 | 53篇 |
1992年 | 44篇 |
1991年 | 38篇 |
1990年 | 34篇 |
1989年 | 37篇 |
1988年 | 42篇 |
1987年 | 46篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 47篇 |
1984年 | 42篇 |
1983年 | 36篇 |
1982年 | 58篇 |
1981年 | 43篇 |
1980年 | 38篇 |
1979年 | 34篇 |
1978年 | 46篇 |
1977年 | 48篇 |
1976年 | 49篇 |
1975年 | 41篇 |
1974年 | 34篇 |
1973年 | 27篇 |
排序方式: 共有4512条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
61.
Is the size of a semantic-priming effect a function of the strength of the semantic context? This issue was examined in four studies using a single categorically related prime as the weaker context and four categorically related primes as the stronger context. Results indicate that, independently of prime-target SOA, four primes provide a larger priming effect than a single prime in a lexical-decision task, but not in a naming task. These data provide further support for the argument that different mechanisms mediate priming in the two tasks. In particular, only the lexical-decision task appears to be susceptible to higher-level processes that can be influenced by the number of primes. Priming in a naming task appears to be driven by more automatic processes. Possible accounts of this multiple-prime advantage in lexical-decision tasks are considered. 相似文献
62.
63.
Virginia Blankenship Stephen S. Schorie Angela J. Shaw Justin Tumlinson 《Behavior research methods》1994,26(2):128-133
Although the Rescorla-Wagner model is widely taught in learning courses, most textbooks present a simplified version that does not contain all of its parameters. On the basis of Rescorla and Wagner 1972) and the Turbo PASCAL program by Lachnit, Schneider, Lipp, and Kimme! (1988), we created a simulation using STELLA-II, an icon-based program by High Performance Systems, Inc. We argue that this simulation facilitates teaching the complete Rescorla-Wagner model. Using trans-switching (Kimmel & Ray, 1978; Lachnit, 1986; Murrin & Kimmel, 1986) as an example, we demonstrate the effectiveness of the STELLA-II version of the Rescorla-Wagner model as a heuristic tool. 相似文献
64.
A graphical analysis similar to that used by Hinton and Shallice (1991) was applied to the hair cells of a simulated auditory transducer. The graphical analysis made it apparent that there were hair cells that responded to a narrow range of frequencies, as would be predicted by the tonotopic organization of the real physiology. In short, this study demonstrates the efficacy of using graphic techniques to examine the nature of the autopoetic organization of the hidden layer of back-propagation networks. 相似文献
65.
Charles Mulford Motoko Y. Lee Stephen C. Sapp 《Journal of applied social psychology》1996,26(15):1324-1336
Until now, no general scales have been available for measuring victim or society blaming. This constraint has been an impediment to policymaking with regard to those who are affected by social problems. Four scales were constructed: stable victim-blaming scale, unstable victim-blaming scale, stable society-blaming scale, and unstable society-blaming scale. Based on the results of the preliminary exploratory factor analysis and the reliability coefficients, the unstable society-blaming scale was deleted from further analysis. Then, a confirmatory factor analysis using the LISREL 7 program was conducted to evaluate the parameter estimates of the scale items and examine the construct validity of the 3 scales. The scales were judged to have construct validity with satisfactory reliability. 相似文献
66.
John M. Roll Stephen T. Higgins Gary J. Badger 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1996,29(4):495-505
The efficacy of three different schedules of reinforcement for promoting and sustaining drug abstinence was compared in this study. Cigarette smoking was studied as an exemplar of stimulant drug self-administration. Sixty cigarette smokers were assigned to one of three groups (progressive rate of reinforcement, fixed rate of reinforcement, and yoked control). Participants in all three groups were asked to refrain from smoking for 1 week. Participants in the progressive and fixed groups achieved greater mean levels of abstinence than those in the control group. Participants in the progressive group were significantly less likely to resume smoking when they became abstinent than participants in the other groups. 相似文献
67.
Dianne Parker Stephen G. Stradling Antony S. R. Manstead 《Journal of applied social psychology》1996,26(1):1-19
This study builds on previous theory of planned behavior (TPB) studies in which we identified the beliefs and values which predict intention to commit driving violations. Four short experimental videos were developed in order to assess the effectiveness of an intervention grounded in the TPB. Three of the videos featured the major constructs of the TPB model. The fourth video featured anticipated regret, an addition to the TPB model which had previously been shown to add significantly to its predictive performance (Parker, Manstead, & Stradling, 1995). Results indicated that two of the videos brought about statistically significant belief changes with respect to scores on TPB items, and significant changes in general attitudes toward speeding. Discussion centers on the problems encountered in operationalizing the TPB constructs and on the potential of theory-based interventions to induce attitude change. 相似文献
68.
Tasks and timing in the perception of linguistic anomaly 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Janet Dean Fodor Weijia Ni Stephen Crain Donald Shankweiler 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1996,25(1):25-57
Three experiments were conducted to investigate the relative timing of syntactic and pragmatic anomaly detection during sentence processing. Experiment 1 was an eye movement study. Experiment 2 employed a dual-task paradigm with compressed speech input, to put the processing routines under time pressure. Experiment 3 used compressed speech input in an anomaly monitoring task. The outcomes of these experiments suggest that there is little or no delay in pragmatic processing relative to syntactic processing in the comprehension of unambiguous sentences. This narrows the possible explanations for any delays that are observed in the use of pragmatic information for ambiguity resolution.The research reported in this paper was supported by Program Project Grant HD-01994 to Haskins Laboratories from the National Institutes of Child and Human Development, and in part by a grant to Stephen Crain and Donald Shankweiler from the University of Connecticut Research Foundation. We thank Jacques Mehler and Gerry Altmann for helpful information about speech compression algorithms. We are especially grateful to Ignatius Mattingly for his generous expert advice on speech compression and design of the experimental materials, and also for recording all the speech stimuli. Julie Boland, Brian McElree, Janet Nicol, and Martin Pickering provided very helpful advice on an earlier draft. 相似文献
69.
The health belief model was evaluated for its ability to predict perceived and actual dietary quality. The assessment relied upon data collected from 1,502 respondents by the US. Department of Agriculture in its 1991 Continuing Survey of Food Intakes by Individuals and the Diet and Health Knowledge Survey. The health belief model provided good prediction of perceived dietary quality, but provided moderate to weak prediction of 7 measures of actual dietary quality that were calculated from food intake records. Implications are suggested for using the health belief model to understand the determinants of dietary quality. 相似文献
70.
Peter F. W. PreeceKenneth L. Q. Read 《Intelligence》1996,23(3):229
Based on the construct of classical IQ (mental age/chronological age), a model of the proportions of the population at various stages of cognitive development as a function of age is proposed. The model, which accounts well for empirical data for the age range 13 through 17 years, is shown to compare favorably with other theoretical models. The success of the model provides further evidence of the salience of the construct of general cognitive capacity in differential psychology. 相似文献