首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4822篇
  免费   198篇
  5020篇
  2022年   27篇
  2021年   33篇
  2020年   74篇
  2019年   64篇
  2018年   109篇
  2017年   121篇
  2016年   173篇
  2015年   80篇
  2014年   124篇
  2013年   627篇
  2012年   178篇
  2011年   203篇
  2010年   111篇
  2009年   153篇
  2008年   196篇
  2007年   199篇
  2006年   202篇
  2005年   130篇
  2004年   167篇
  2003年   165篇
  2002年   149篇
  2001年   86篇
  2000年   88篇
  1999年   71篇
  1998年   77篇
  1997年   69篇
  1996年   64篇
  1995年   59篇
  1994年   76篇
  1993年   56篇
  1992年   49篇
  1991年   46篇
  1990年   38篇
  1989年   40篇
  1988年   52篇
  1987年   52篇
  1986年   41篇
  1985年   52篇
  1984年   47篇
  1983年   40篇
  1982年   58篇
  1981年   47篇
  1980年   40篇
  1979年   48篇
  1978年   51篇
  1977年   51篇
  1976年   54篇
  1975年   46篇
  1974年   46篇
  1973年   41篇
排序方式: 共有5020条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
A random utility model of choice was developed by combining the basic ideas of the well-known theories of Thurstone and Restle. The new model has exactly the same number of free parameters as Tversky's Elimination-by-Aspects model. Furthermore, both models were found to fit, with equal accuracy, the data reported by Rumelhart and Greeno, and Tversky. It was concluded that although the two theories are not identical, they may be difficult to discriminate empirically.  相似文献   
132.
Subsequent to success or failure experience, 48 subjects attempted a psychomotor task in order to escape or avoid an aversive stimulus (tone). Their attempts at solving the task over 24 trials were made in the presence of subject-experimenters each of whom had expectancies instilled regarding the solution to the task. The job of the subject-experimenters was to “offer information” on every third trials as the subjects attempted to solve the task. It was hypothesized that the expectancies of the subject-experimenters would be communicated to the subjects via periodic comments, that these expectancies would differentially affect the subjects' performance on the task, and that prior experience on a similar task would differentially affect subjects' performances on the task. The results demonstrated that the expectancies instilled in the subject-experimenters were communicated and influenced the performances of subjects as predicted. It appears that consistency with respect to the expectancies communicated played a major role in producing significant effects. Prior experience failed to produce significant differences on task performance, a finding conflicting with that of D.S. Hiroto (Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974, 102, 187–193). The results are discussed from the perspective of using the overt, explicit communication of expectancies as a means for exploring the illusive phenomenon of the experimenter expectancy effect.  相似文献   
133.
134.
135.
136.
One criticism of Horner's fear-of-success (FOS) research concerns the fact that she defined success (being number 1 in a medical school class) for her subjects. Two aims of the present investigation were to (a) reinstate the traditional ambiguity of the projective verbal lead: “Anne (John) has succeeded,” and (b) determine the amount of FOS imagery projected by 64 male and 79 female college students to these leads. The low incidence of FOS in this present study was attributed to the opportunity subjects were given to define success for themselves. There were no significant differences in the amount of FOS projected by men and women. School-related success was the most often cited success theme. Results were supportive of the hypothesis that FOS is situationally determined.  相似文献   
137.
Crisis counselling, whilst closely related to long-term counselling, involves a variety of processes and procedures which make it a distinctive counselling form. The implications of this distinctiveness for counsellor training and education are examined. These implications are highlighted in a case-study of the training of Samaritans.  相似文献   
138.
139.
The concept of family myth can be used by the therapist to help organize the wealth of information provided by a family in therapy. Rituals, symbols and metaphors can be viewed as structurally related components within the family myth system which enable the therapist to interpret non-literal, analogic communications as a coherent yet primarily symbolic, narrative about the family's shared perceptions of its functioning. The concept of myth is explored in relation to mythology, the family therapy literature, and a case study. Several strategic intervention strategies are proposed which utilize the family's own metaphors, symbols and rituals to re-edit the family myth. Re-editing the myth on a symbolic level should be associated with improved system functioning, including more concrete areas of the family's life.  相似文献   
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号