全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4218篇 |
免费 | 175篇 |
专业分类
4393篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 66篇 |
2019年 | 54篇 |
2018年 | 93篇 |
2017年 | 106篇 |
2016年 | 158篇 |
2015年 | 70篇 |
2014年 | 117篇 |
2013年 | 553篇 |
2012年 | 162篇 |
2011年 | 184篇 |
2010年 | 100篇 |
2009年 | 141篇 |
2008年 | 176篇 |
2007年 | 177篇 |
2006年 | 191篇 |
2005年 | 117篇 |
2004年 | 159篇 |
2003年 | 138篇 |
2002年 | 136篇 |
2001年 | 69篇 |
2000年 | 64篇 |
1999年 | 59篇 |
1998年 | 69篇 |
1997年 | 63篇 |
1996年 | 56篇 |
1995年 | 51篇 |
1994年 | 71篇 |
1993年 | 51篇 |
1992年 | 42篇 |
1991年 | 37篇 |
1990年 | 32篇 |
1989年 | 36篇 |
1988年 | 40篇 |
1987年 | 44篇 |
1986年 | 32篇 |
1985年 | 46篇 |
1984年 | 42篇 |
1983年 | 35篇 |
1982年 | 56篇 |
1981年 | 42篇 |
1980年 | 37篇 |
1979年 | 34篇 |
1978年 | 45篇 |
1977年 | 48篇 |
1976年 | 49篇 |
1975年 | 41篇 |
1974年 | 33篇 |
1973年 | 27篇 |
排序方式: 共有4393条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Stephen G. Weinrach 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1995,73(3):296-300
Objections to Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) seem to exceed the mere rational preference for one approach over another. Ziegler suggested that James's dichotomy between Tough- and Tender-Mindedness might explain REBT's appeal to some and its failure to attract others. REBT is a predominantly Tough-Minded therapy, but the counseling profession is largely Tender Minded. In this article, the author examines why Tender-Minded counselors may not accept REBT, what common misperceptions of REBT may contribute to this rejection, and how the two might be reconciled. Intervention strategies for Tender-Minded counselors are suggested. 相似文献
52.
53.
Robert F. Belli D. Stephen Lindsay Maria S. Gales Thomas T. McCarthy 《Memory & cognition》1994,22(1):40-54
The four experiments reported here provide evidence that (1) misleading postevent suggestions can impair memory for details in a witnessed event and (2) subjects sometimes remember sug-gested details as things seen in the event itself. All four experiments used recall tests in which subjects were warned of the possibility that the postevent information included misleading sug-gestions and were instructed to report both what they witnessed in the event and what was men-tioned in the postevent narrative. Recall of event details was poorer on misled items than on control items, and subjects sometimes misidentified the sources of their recollections. Our re-sults suggest that these findings are not due to guessing or response biases, but rather reflect genuine memory impairment and source monitoring confusions. 相似文献
54.
Is the size of a semantic-priming effect a function of the strength of the semantic context? This issue was examined in four studies using a single categorically related prime as the weaker context and four categorically related primes as the stronger context. Results indicate that, independently of prime-target SOA, four primes provide a larger priming effect than a single prime in a lexical-decision task, but not in a naming task. These data provide further support for the argument that different mechanisms mediate priming in the two tasks. In particular, only the lexical-decision task appears to be susceptible to higher-level processes that can be influenced by the number of primes. Priming in a naming task appears to be driven by more automatic processes. Possible accounts of this multiple-prime advantage in lexical-decision tasks are considered. 相似文献
55.
56.
Virginia Blankenship Stephen S. Schorie Angela J. Shaw Justin Tumlinson 《Behavior research methods》1994,26(2):128-133
Although the Rescorla-Wagner model is widely taught in learning courses, most textbooks present a simplified version that does not contain all of its parameters. On the basis of Rescorla and Wagner 1972) and the Turbo PASCAL program by Lachnit, Schneider, Lipp, and Kimme! (1988), we created a simulation using STELLA-II, an icon-based program by High Performance Systems, Inc. We argue that this simulation facilitates teaching the complete Rescorla-Wagner model. Using trans-switching (Kimmel & Ray, 1978; Lachnit, 1986; Murrin & Kimmel, 1986) as an example, we demonstrate the effectiveness of the STELLA-II version of the Rescorla-Wagner model as a heuristic tool. 相似文献
57.
A graphical analysis similar to that used by Hinton and Shallice (1991) was applied to the hair cells of a simulated auditory transducer. The graphical analysis made it apparent that there were hair cells that responded to a narrow range of frequencies, as would be predicted by the tonotopic organization of the real physiology. In short, this study demonstrates the efficacy of using graphic techniques to examine the nature of the autopoetic organization of the hidden layer of back-propagation networks. 相似文献
58.
Charles Mulford Motoko Y. Lee Stephen C. Sapp 《Journal of applied social psychology》1996,26(15):1324-1336
Until now, no general scales have been available for measuring victim or society blaming. This constraint has been an impediment to policymaking with regard to those who are affected by social problems. Four scales were constructed: stable victim-blaming scale, unstable victim-blaming scale, stable society-blaming scale, and unstable society-blaming scale. Based on the results of the preliminary exploratory factor analysis and the reliability coefficients, the unstable society-blaming scale was deleted from further analysis. Then, a confirmatory factor analysis using the LISREL 7 program was conducted to evaluate the parameter estimates of the scale items and examine the construct validity of the 3 scales. The scales were judged to have construct validity with satisfactory reliability. 相似文献
59.
John M. Roll Stephen T. Higgins Gary J. Badger 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1996,29(4):495-505
The efficacy of three different schedules of reinforcement for promoting and sustaining drug abstinence was compared in this study. Cigarette smoking was studied as an exemplar of stimulant drug self-administration. Sixty cigarette smokers were assigned to one of three groups (progressive rate of reinforcement, fixed rate of reinforcement, and yoked control). Participants in all three groups were asked to refrain from smoking for 1 week. Participants in the progressive and fixed groups achieved greater mean levels of abstinence than those in the control group. Participants in the progressive group were significantly less likely to resume smoking when they became abstinent than participants in the other groups. 相似文献
60.
Dianne Parker Stephen G. Stradling Antony S. R. Manstead 《Journal of applied social psychology》1996,26(1):1-19
This study builds on previous theory of planned behavior (TPB) studies in which we identified the beliefs and values which predict intention to commit driving violations. Four short experimental videos were developed in order to assess the effectiveness of an intervention grounded in the TPB. Three of the videos featured the major constructs of the TPB model. The fourth video featured anticipated regret, an addition to the TPB model which had previously been shown to add significantly to its predictive performance (Parker, Manstead, & Stradling, 1995). Results indicated that two of the videos brought about statistically significant belief changes with respect to scores on TPB items, and significant changes in general attitudes toward speeding. Discussion centers on the problems encountered in operationalizing the TPB constructs and on the potential of theory-based interventions to induce attitude change. 相似文献