首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   41570篇
  免费   1547篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2018年   678篇
  2017年   633篇
  2016年   760篇
  2015年   519篇
  2014年   646篇
  2013年   4054篇
  2012年   1068篇
  2011年   1121篇
  2010年   701篇
  2009年   720篇
  2008年   1083篇
  2007年   982篇
  2006年   958篇
  2005年   763篇
  2004年   766篇
  2003年   770篇
  2002年   750篇
  2001年   1264篇
  2000年   1221篇
  1999年   951篇
  1998年   462篇
  1996年   564篇
  1995年   520篇
  1994年   530篇
  1993年   496篇
  1992年   963篇
  1991年   847篇
  1990年   857篇
  1989年   748篇
  1988年   768篇
  1987年   748篇
  1986年   732篇
  1985年   738篇
  1984年   652篇
  1983年   544篇
  1982年   447篇
  1981年   452篇
  1979年   621篇
  1978年   477篇
  1976年   449篇
  1975年   583篇
  1974年   566篇
  1973年   621篇
  1972年   587篇
  1971年   530篇
  1970年   458篇
  1969年   444篇
  1968年   558篇
  1967年   469篇
  1966年   456篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
Self‐abandonment and self‐denial are, respectively, Catholic and hyper‐Calvinist analogues of each other. Roughly, each requires the surrendering of a person to God's will and providence through faith, hope, and love. Should the self‐abandoning/self‐denying individual accept his or her own damnation if that be God's will? This article, which is virtually alone in discussing the Catholic and Reformed Protestant traditions together, answers “No.” The unqualified self‐abandonment present in quietism and the radical self‐denial of Samuel Hopkins are perverse and irrational responses to the prospect of hell because they run counter to the Christian's deepest need to spend eternity with God. However, a qualified self‐abandonment is intellectually defensible and offers a viable Christian piety.  相似文献   
59.
60.
In five experiments, we examined 3‐ to 6‐year‐olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge indirectly from someone who had seen something they had not. Consistent with previous research, children judged that an informant, who had seen inside a box, knew its contents. Similarly, when an informant marked a picture to indicate her suggestion as to the content of the box, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds trusted this more frequently when the informant had seen inside the box than when she had not. Going beyond previous research, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds were also sensitive to informants’ relevant experience when they had to look over a barrier to see the marked picture, or ask for the barrier to be raised. Yet when children had to elicit the informant's suggestion, rather than just consult a suggestion already present, even 4‐ to 5‐year‐olds were no more likely to do so when the informant had seen the box's content than when she had not, and no more likely to trust the well‐informed suggestion than the uninformed one. We conclude that young children who can ask questions may not yet fully understand the process by which they can gain accurate information from someone who has the experience they lack.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号