首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1282篇
  免费   93篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   12篇
  2021年   19篇
  2020年   45篇
  2019年   50篇
  2018年   66篇
  2017年   68篇
  2016年   80篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   48篇
  2013年   162篇
  2012年   88篇
  2011年   69篇
  2010年   45篇
  2009年   40篇
  2008年   75篇
  2007年   59篇
  2006年   55篇
  2005年   51篇
  2004年   43篇
  2003年   40篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   17篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   12篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   1篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   4篇
排序方式: 共有1375条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
In his Investigations, Wittgenstein employed a quotation from Augustine to capture certain of the essential features of an incoherent conception of language that he believed was at root of many of the dominant theories of meaning of his day. It is argued in the current paper that this very same Augustinian conception of language (ACL) is the foundation of some of the most influential methodological orientations of present-day psychological science, and, as a result, these orientations suffer from a range of ACL-induced incoherences. This thesis is illustrated by way of a case study drawn from the construct validation literature.  相似文献   
992.
Caregiving Behavior Is Associated With Decreased Mortality Risk   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
ABSTRACT— Traditional investigations of caregiving link it to increased caregiver morbidity and mortality, but do not disentangle the effects of providing care from those of being continuously exposed to an ailing loved one with serious health problems. We explored this possible confound in a national, longitudinal survey of elderly married individuals ( N = 3,376). Results showed that spending at least 14 hr per week providing care to a spouse predicted decreased mortality for the caregiver, independently of behavioral and cognitive limitations of the care recipient (spouse), and of other demographic and health variables. These findings suggest that it may be premature to conclude that health risks for caregivers are due to providing active help. Indeed, under some circumstances, caregivers may actually benefit from providing care.  相似文献   
993.
In order to evaluate the utility of genetic counseling at the time of first trimester screening in patients with no previously identified genetic concerns, we reviewed family history data for 700 women seen for genetic counseling in Utah during 2005-2006. The mean maternal age was 35 years (Range: 16–47 years). The majority of patients seen were non-Jewish Caucasians (90.8%, 634/700). A three-generation pedigree was obtained from each woman by one of two certified genetic counselors and subsequently classified as “negative” (no birth defects/genetic disorders); “positive” (birth defect or genetic condition with a minimal/low risk of recurrence; additional evaluation/genetic testing during pregnancy not indicated); or “significant” (birth defect or genetic condition with an increased risk of recurrence; additional evaluation/genetic testing during the pregnancy indicated). About 72% (501/700) of the histories were negative, 19% (134/700) were positive, and about 9% (65/700) were significant. Among patients with significant family histories, 66% (n = 43) were women less than 35 years of age. We conclude that assessing a patient’s family history at the time of first trimester serum screening is a valuable resource for pregnancy management.  相似文献   
994.
This study examined how emerging adults’ identity development and achievement of adulthood criteria were related to qualities of their friendships and romantic relationships. Participants included 710 emerging adults (ages 18–26). Results indicated that identity achievement was related positively to four romantic relationship qualities, but not to any friendship qualities. Several achieved adulthood criteria were related positively to romantic relationship qualities; however, achieved adulthood criteria were related negatively to friendship qualities. It appears that progress on salient developmental tasks of adulthood carries important implications for emerging adults’ social relationships, but in ways that are more differentiated than commonly assumed.  相似文献   
995.
It is widely believed that a descending serial circuit consisting of neural projections from the basolateral complex (BLA) to the central nucleus (CEA) of the amygdala mediates fear expression. Here we directly test this hypothesis and show that disconnecting the BLA and CEA with asymmetric neurotoxic lesions after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats completely abolishes the expression of conditional freezing. These results demonstrate that neural projections from the BLA to CEA are essential for the expression of learned fear responses.Long-standing anatomical models of the brain circuitry underlying learned fear posit serial flow of information through the amygdala to engage the expression of fear responses (LeDoux 2000; Maren 2001). Specifically, conditioning-induced plasticity in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA) is thought to drive learned fear through excitatory axonal projections to the basal nuclei of the amygdala (BA; basolateral and basomedial nuclei), which in turn send unidirectional and excitatory synaptic projections to the medial division of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CEm). Neurons in the CEm project to brain structures involved in the production of a variety of fear responses (Pitkänen et al. 1997; Swanson and Petrovich 1998). Alternatively, neurons in LA might excite neurons in CEm by limiting inhibitory input from the intercalated cell masses (ITC) interposed between the basolateral complex (BLA; lateral and basal nuclei) and the central nucleus of the amygdala (CEA) (Paré et al. 2004). In either case, the BLA is well positioned to drive learned fear responses via anatomical connections with the CEA.Although an extensive literature demonstrates the importance of both the BLA and CEA in the acquisition and expression of fear (LeDoux et al. 1990; Lee et al. 1996; Amorapanth et al. 2000; Goosens and Maren 2001), it is not known whether a functional connection between the two structures is essential for the expression of learned fear. Indeed, the BLA and CEA make independent contributions to aversively motivated learning under some conditions (Killcross et al. 1997; Amorapanth et al. 2000). Moreover, recent work in appetitive conditioning paradigms challenges the necessity of serial circuits in the amygdala for associative learning processes (Holland and Gallagher 1999; Everitt et al. 2003; Balleine and Killcross 2006). It is therefore essential to determine whether serial connections between the BLA and CEA are involved in the expression of fear memories as widely assumed in the literature.To address this issue, we made asymmetric neurotoxic lesions of the BLA and CEA after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. That is, we placed BLA lesions in one hemisphere and CEA lesions in the contralateral hemisphere, thereby producing a functional disconnection of the two brain regions. Control animals received neurotoxic BLA and CEA lesions in the same hemisphere, thereby leaving both structures and their connections intact in one hemisphere. This disconnection strategy (e.g., Olton et al. 1982) capitalizes on the fact that projections from the BLA to CEA are both ipsilateral and unidirectional (Pitkänen et al. 1997). It has been used by several groups to assess the contribution of connections between brain areas to learning and memory processes, including conditioned stimulus processing (Han et al. 1999) and appetitive spatial learning (Ito et al. 2008), for example.Fear conditioning was conducted in standard observation chambers (see Supplemental Methods) and consisted of five pairings of an auditory conditional stimulus (CS) (2 kHz, 10 sec, 80 dB) with a footshock unconditioned stimulus (US) (2 sec, 1 mA); the intertrial interval (ITI) was 1 min. Freezing behavior served as the measure of conditional fear. Twenty-four hours after fear conditioning, the rats were deeply anesthetized, and amygdala lesions were made by infusing N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA; 20 mg/mL in 100 mM phosphate buffered saline with a pH 7.4; Sigma) into the CEA and BLA through a 28-g injection cannula attached to a Hamilton syringe via polyethylene tubing. Control rats received unilateral lesions of the BLA and CEA in the same hemisphere (IPSI, n = 13) or sham surgery (SHAM, n = 16), whereas experimental rats received the unilateral BLA and CEA lesions in opposite hemispheres (CONTRA, n = 11). Hence, functional connectivity between the BLA and CEA was left intact in one hemisphere among rats in the IPSI group, whereas the asymmetric lesions in rats in the CONTRA group eliminated this connection. Retention tests assessing fear to the conditioning context and tone CS were conducted in separate sessions one week after recovery from surgery. Histological examination of coronal brain sections obtained from the subjects after the experiment revealed selective CEA and BLA lesions in each group; representative lesions are illustrated in Figure 1, A and B. The lesions spared fibers of passage (Fig. 1C).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Disconnection of the BLA and CEA impairs the expression of conditional fear. (A) Schematic representation of a typical BLA (shaded) and CEA (black) lesions among rats with unilateral lesions placed in the same hemisphere (IPSI) or in opposite hemispheres (CONTRA). (B) Photomicrograph of a thionin-stained coronal section from a representative rat in the CONTRA group. The image has been cropped to include only the left and right amygdale. Broken lines encircle CEA and BLA lesions in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. (C, left) Photomicrograph of a thionin-stained coronal section from a representative rat in the IPSI group. The image has been cropped to focus on the lesion in the right amygdala. Broken lines encircle the CEA and BLA lesions. (C, right) Photomicrograph of an AuCl-stained coronal section adjacent to that shown at left indicates that NMDA infusions into the amygdala did not damage myelinated axons in the vicinity of the lesion. (D) Mean percentage of freezing (± SEM) during the conditioning session (collapsed across CSs and ITIs) and retention tests in rats in each of the three groups. The expression of fear to the conditioning context and tone CS was severely impaired by disconnection of the BLA and CEA in the CONTRA group. *P < 0.05.As shown in Figure 1D (left panel), all rats acquired similar levels of conditional freezing (collapsed across the CS and ITI for each trial) by the end of the presurgical fear conditioning session (main effect of trial, F(5,185) = 42.1, P < 0.0001); there was neither a main effect of group (F < 1) nor a group × trial interaction (F < 1.3). One week after recovery from surgery, conditional freezing to the conditioning context and the auditory CS was assessed in separate retention tests. Importantly, functional disconnection of the BLA and the CEA after fear conditioning severely impaired the expression of conditioned freezing to both the conditioning context and the auditory CS (Fig. 1D, right). Indeed, the expression of fear to the auditory CS was essentially abolished. Freezing among rats in the CONTRA group was significantly lower than that in both the SHAM and IPSI groups during both retention tests (main effect of group, F(2,37) = 9.98, P < 0.001), and this difference did not interact with the nature of the retention test (F < 1). Hence, functional disconnection of the BLA and CEA produced an impairment in the expression of conditional fear that was much greater than that produced by comparable lesions that spared this connection.The logic of the disconnection procedure requires that focal CEA and BLA lesions in each hemisphere do not damage the adjacent BLA or CEA, respectively. For instance, if CEA lesions produce retrograde degeneration in the neighboring BLA, rats in the CONTRA group would effectively have bilateral lesions in the BLA. Such an outcome would be expected to yield the massive deficits in conditional freezing that we have observed. Inspection of thionin-stained coronal sections suggested that, as in previous studies (Goosens and Maren 2001), our lesions were indeed selective for the targeted areas (Fig. 1B). However, to increase our confidence that the BLA adjacent to a CEA lesion was in fact functional, we performed c-fos immunohistochemistry on brain sections obtained from a separate group of rats trained and tested as previously described (SHAM, CONTRA, and IPSI groups; n = 8 per group). In addition to these rats, we included a group of rats that were not conditioned (NO-SHOCK, n = 8) to quantify fear-induced increases in c-fos expression. All rats were sacrificed 90 min after the retention test to the auditory CS. Amygdaloid c-fos expression in the SHAM rats and the intact hemisphere of the IPSI rats did not differ, and these groups were therefore collapsed into a single SHAM group.As shown in Figure 2, SHAM rats exhibited a greater density of c-fos positive nuclei in both the CEA and BLA relative to nonshocked controls. Quantification of these data confirmed this observation and revealed significant differences in c-fos expression among the groups in both the CEA (Fig. 2, left; F(2,40) = 5.59, P < 0.01) and the BLA (Fig. 2, right; F(2,40) = 5.07, P < 0.01). Importantly, BLA c-fos expression adjacent to CEA lesions in the CONTRA group was similar to that in intact SHAM rats, and significantly greater than that in nonshocked controls (Fig. 2B, right). This suggests that the failure of CONTRA rats to express conditional fear responses was not due to a failure to engage the intact BLA but rather to the functional disconnection of BLA activity from CEA output. Indeed, the CEA adjacent to a BLA lesion exhibited a marked reduction in c-fos expression relative to SHAM controls (Fig. 2, left), indicating that BLA lesions failed to drive ipsilateral CEA neurons important for the expression of learned fear. It is possible that the absence of c-fos expression in the CEA, in this case, is due to encroachment of the adjacent BLA lesion. However, thionin-stained sections revealed that BLA lesions were selective. Moreover, we observed intact BLA c-fos expression in rats with adjacent CEA lesions suggesting that nearby lesions per se do not disrupt c-fos expression.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Functional disconnection of the BLA and CEA revealed by c-fos expression. Mean density (± SEM) of c-fos positive nuclei in the CEA (left) and BLA (right) in rats from each of the three groups. Disconnection of the BLA and CEA eliminated fear-related increases in c-fos expression in the CEA (left), but not the BLA (right). *P < 0.05.These results reveal that a functional connection between the BLA and CEA is required for the expression of learned fear. Considering that BLA projections to the CEA are largely unidirectional, our data reveal that a serial circuit from the BLA to the CEA mediates the expression of conditional fear responses. Consistent with this view, there are numerous reports that permanent lesions or reversible inactivation of either the BLA or CEA prevent the expression of conditioned fear (Lee et al. 1996; Maren 1999; Zimmerman et al. 2007). It is now apparent that the necessity for both the BLA and CEA in fear expression arises from the functional connectivity between them. Anatomically, this connection might involve a direct excitatory projection from BA to CEm (Paré et al. 1999) or indirect projections from LA to ITC and the lateral division of the central nucleus (CEl), both of which project to CEm (Smith and Paré 1994; Paré et al. 2004). Recent data reveal, however, that selective immunotoxic lesions of the ITC do not impair the expression of conditioned freezing (Likhtik et al. 2008). Moreover, CEl projections to CEm are inhibitory, making it unlikely that that an LA-CEl projection drives learned fear responses via CEm (Paré et al. 2004). Hence, it appears that the most likely route by which fear CSs drive learned fear involves projections from BA to CEm. Consistent with this, selective BA lesions disrupt the expression of conditioned freezing when made either before (Goosens and Maren 2001) or after (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk 2005) fear conditioning.The dependence of conditional fear on a serial circuit between the BLA and CEA stands in contrast to the independent roles these areas have been proposed to play in appetitive conditioning paradigms (Holland and Gallagher 1999; Everitt et al. 2003; Balleine and Killcross 2006). For example, CEA, but not BLA, lesions have been reported to produce deficits in the acquisition and expression of autoshaped conditioned responses (CRs), indicating that the CEA has an independent contribution to CR expression for food-motivated responses (Parkinson et al. 2000). Moreover, the BLA has a role in the attribution of incentive salience to rewarding stimuli independent of the generation of CRs to those stimuli (Hatfield et al. 1996). However, in aversive conditioning, it appears that the BLA may not encode the motivational properties of the shock US (Rabinak and Maren 2008), but rather CS–US associations that are essential for organizing conditional fear responses by the CEA. Indeed, there is an emerging body of data suggesting that these associations may be established not only in sensory afferents in the BLA but also in the CEA (Wilensky et al. 2006; Zimmerman et al. 2007). Synaptic plasticity in projections from BA to CEm may be the essential substrate underlying the functional connectivity between these structures that is essential for the expression of fear memory.  相似文献   
996.
In addressing the call for research to understand affect in sales, this research uses moral affect theory and literature on emotional contagion to examine the effects of salesperson gratitude and customer gratitude on downstream relationship outcomes. The findings of this work suggest that salesperson prosocial behaviors account for the positive association between salesperson gratitude and customer gratitude. These prosocial behaviors – information sharing and extra-role behaviors – combined with customer gratitude serve as explanatory mechanisms for the positive effects of salesperson gratitude on customer commitment, which is an important result of buyer-seller interactions and an essential component of long-term relationships. Further, this research finds that salesperson extra-role behaviors and relationship length interact such that salesperson extra-role behaviors cultivate customer gratitude within developing and established relationships, but that these behaviors are particularly beneficial for less mature relationships. Understanding the role of salesperson gratitude and customer gratitude in driving relational outcomes contributes to both a theoretical understanding of the role of affect in sales and practical applications of emotions within buyer-seller relationships.  相似文献   
997.
Two experiments investigated the effects of age and domain-specific experience on the speed and accuracy of visual inspection performance. In Experiment 1, young (M age = 26.5 years) and middle-aged (M age = 45.7 years) medical laboratory technologists (MTs) and matched novices were tested on a domain-specific version and on a domain-general version of a probe recognition task. Middle-aged subjects were slower than younger subjects on both versions, and MTs were more accurate but slower than controls on the domain-specific task. In Experiment 2, MTs and controls were tested on the same tasks under single-task and dual-task conditions. Middle-aged adults were slower and less accurate than young adults under dual-task conditions in the general version. For the domain-specific version, the response times and error data suggested that skilled performance is less demanding of age-limited general-purpose processing resources.  相似文献   
998.
Women tend to be portrayed in a sexualized or domestic manner in mainstream advertising; importantly this trend holds not only for ads targeting men but also for those targeting women themselves. Such a focus on sexualized portrayals in particular may not seem strategic given a wealth of evidence suggesting that women evaluate these portrayals quite negatively. Consumer attitudes toward domestic portrayals are more mixed but, unsurprisingly, vary according to how much a woman identifies as traditional. If female consumers do not evaluate these common portrayals positively, why might they persist? Past work suggests a disconnect between reported attitudes toward general visual sexual stimuli and physiological and neural responses; therefore, it is plausible that neural responses to stereotypical female portrayals in advertising may be at odds with reported attitudes and may have a bigger impact on consumer behavior. The current study exposed women to sexualized, domestic, and control images in a functional magnetic resonance imaging scanner as an initial test of this idea. We found that participants reported liking both domestic and control images more than sexualized images. In contrast, they showed more activity in regions associated with reward and arousal (ventral striatum and amygdala, respectively) while viewing sexualized images relative to both control and domestic images. Surprisingly, ventral striatum response to sexualized ads was stronger for women who endorsed traditional attitudes than those who reported less traditional attitudes. These results suggest that despite reporting negative attitudes toward sexualized portrayals, women may in fact have a favorable response to these images. Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
999.
1000.
We evaluated a unique procedure to establish compliance with instructions in four young children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who had low levels of compliance. Our procedure included methods to establish a novel therapist as a source of positive reinforcement, reliably evoke orienting responses to the therapist, increase the number of exposures to instruction–compliance–reinforcer contingencies, and minimize the number of exposures to instruction–noncompliance–no reinforcer contingencies. We further alternated between instructions with a high probability of compliance (high‐p instructions) with instructions that had a prior low probability of compliance (low‐p instructions) as soon as low‐p instructions lost stimulus control. The intervention is discussed in relation to the conditions necessary for the development of stimulus control and as an example of a variation of translational research.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号