全文获取类型
收费全文 | 725篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
730篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 72篇 |
2012年 | 52篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 38篇 |
2007年 | 39篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 15篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有730条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
Krøjgaard P 《Developmental science》2007,10(6):892-909
Discussions have recently taken place on whether spatiotemporal information is more important than featural information when infants attempt to individuate objects. Hitherto, spatiotemporal and featural information have only been compared directly by using cognitively demanding 'event-mapping designs' (e.g. Xu & Carey, 1996), whereas the simpler event-monitoring designs (e.g. the 'wide-screen/narrow-screen' by Wilcox and colleagues) have not been employed for such a comparison. The present research offers a new event-monitoring design, the rotating screen design, that allows for such a direct comparison. Three experiments in which 9.5-, 8.0-, and 6.5-month-old infants attempt to individuate objects by spatiotemporal and featural information are reported. The results showed that whereas the 9.5-month-old infants were able to individuate objects by spatiotemporal as well as featural information, the infants of the younger age groups only successfully individuated objects when provided with spatiotemporal information, but not with featural information. 相似文献
142.
143.
Stefanie Kehrer Antje Kraft Kerstin Irlbacher Stefan P. Koch Herbert Hagendorf Norbert Kathmann Stephan A. Brandt 《Psychological research》2009,73(6):751-761
Event-related potentials were measured to investigate the role of visual spatial attention mechanisms in conflict processing.
We suggested that a more difficult target selection leads to stronger attentional top-down control, thereby reducing the effects
of arising conflicts. This hypothesis was tested by varying the selection difficulty in a location negative priming (NP) paradigm.
The difficult task resulted in prolonged responses as compared to the easy task. A behavioral NP effect was only evident in
the easy task. Psychophysiologically the easy task was associated with reduced parietal N1, enhanced frontocentral N2 and
N2pc components and a prolonged P3 latency for the conflict as compared to the control condition. The N2pc effect was also
obvious in the difficult task. Additionally frontocentral N2 amplitudes increased and latencies of N2pc and P3 were delayed
compared to the easy task. The differences at frontocentral and parietal electrodes are consistent with previous studies ascribing
activity in the prefrontal and parietal cortex as the source of top-down attentional control. Thus, we propose that stronger
cognitive control is involved in the difficult task, resulting in a reduced behavioral NP conflict. 相似文献
144.
Visual scanpath recording was used to investigate the information processing strategies used by a prosopagnosic patient, SC, when viewing faces. Compared to controls, SC showed an aberrant pattern of scanning, directing attention away from the internal configuration of facial features (eyes, nose) towards peripheral regions (hair, forehead) of the face. The results suggest that SC's face recognition deficit can be linked to an inability to assemble an accurate and unified face percept due to an abnormal allocation of attention away from the internal face region. Extraction of stimulus attributes necessary for face identity recognition is compromised by an aberrant face scanning pattern. 相似文献
145.
Huijbregts SC Séguin JR Zoccolillo M Boivin M Tremblay RE 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(2):203-215
This study investigated associations between maternal prenatal smoking and physical aggression (PA), hyperactivity-impulsivity
(HI) and co-occurring PA and HI between ages 17 and 42 months in a population sample of children born in Québec (Canada) in
1997/1998 (N=1745). Trajectory model estimation showed three distinct developmental patterns for PA and four for HI. Multinomial regression
analyses showed that prenatal smoking significantly predicted children’s likelihood to follow different PA trajectories beyond
the effects of other perinatal factors, parental psychopathology, family functioning and parenting, and socio-economic factors.
However, prenatal smoking was not a significant predictor of HI in a model with the same control variables. Further multinomial
regression analyses showed that, together with gender, presence of siblings and maternal hostile reactive parenting, prenatal
smoking independently predicted co-occurring high PA and high HI compared to low levels of both behaviors, to high PA alone,
and to high HI alone. These results show that maternal prenatal smoking predicts multiple behavior regulation problems in
early childhood. 相似文献
146.
147.
148.
IMPLICIT MEMORY IN THE TACTILE MODALITY: 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Stephan B. Hamann 《Psychological science》1996,7(5):284-288
Abstract— Research on perceptual priming has previously focused exclusively on priming in the visual and auditory modalities. The present study explored whether perceptual priming also extends to the tactile modality. Tactile priming for Braille words was examined in a group of blind participants, using a Braille analogue of the stem-completion task. The results for tactile priming paralleled previous stem-completion results in other modalities. Manipulating the encoding task at study (semantic vs nonsemantic) dissociated implicit and explicit Braille stem-completion performance, and priming was unaffected by the number of study presentations (one vs three). Finally, Braille stem-completion priming was found in a cross-modal paradigm to have both a specifically tactile component and a cross-modal component. These results demonstrate for the first time that verbal priming can occur in the tactile domain and that tactile priming has basic functional similarities with stem-completion priming in the visual and auditory domains. 相似文献
149.
Decomposed matrix games were used to study interethnic competition in the triethnic school system of a southwestern city. The sample consisted of sixth-grade males from segragated schools. In addition to the data from the matrix games, brief questionnaire measures of internal vs. external locus of control, self-esteem, and authoritarianism were also obtained. The results on the game matrices indicated that the anglos competed more and were less trusting than the blacks or chicanos. The questionnaire results indicated that the blacks were highest on feelings of external control and on authoritarianism, while the chicanos were lowest in self-esteem. Cooperative interethnic work groups were suggested as a means of coping with the problems that these differences might be expected to cause in integrated schools. 相似文献
150.
Side-effects and complications of lithium therapy were examined by evaluation of data registered from 73 patients. Transitory subjective complaints would often occur during the initial phase of therapy, but they do not require any additional therapy. Occasional development of struma and sometimes high increase of body weight require regular controls of these values. Disposing factors concerning the possible occurrence of epileptic seizures and extra-pyramidal disorders under lithium therapy are Melipramin as well as organic cerebral lesions. 相似文献