首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   583篇
  免费   35篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   12篇
  2018年   16篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   17篇
  2015年   18篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   78篇
  2012年   18篇
  2011年   24篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   10篇
  2008年   26篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   30篇
  2005年   15篇
  2004年   21篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   19篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   10篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   6篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   5篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   7篇
  1971年   4篇
  1969年   7篇
  1968年   5篇
  1967年   4篇
  1966年   6篇
排序方式: 共有618条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
541.
Author Index     

Authors Index

Author Index  相似文献   
542.
543.
544.
545.
546.
Using a life course perspective, the research examines personal accounts of adults with schizophrenia, and their parents and well-siblings from six families. Accounts of multiple members of the same family, including the family member with schizophrenia, are used to describe how families understand and accommodate life changes that result from the illness. Families describe the loss of a normal life as one of the most devastating aspects of schizophrenia. We present the personal and social losses described by adults with schizophrenia and their well family members, and document families' search for ways that their ill family member can achieve or maintain valued social roles. The concerns of well family members for the future of the ill family member and ways families contemplate transfer of care issues are described. Implications of the study for community research and action are discussed.  相似文献   
547.
Structural equation models for predictors of traditional family values regarding relationships were examined in 2 samples: undergraduate students and adults ranging widely in age (23-86 years). Predictor variables included verbal ability, need for cognition, need for closure, intolerance for ambiguity, religiosity, and gender orientation. The models accounted for a substantial proportion of the variance in traditional family values (64% for students, 63% for adults). Findings provide little support for common stereotypes regarding age and gender differences in traditionalism. Instead, 3 individual-differences variables predicted traditional family values: need for closure, religiosity, and verbal ability. Outcomes argue for the need to identify multiple mechanisms by which personal characteristics such as need for closure and religiosity influence traditionalism in social belief systems and argue against reliance on status variables such as age and gender as explanatory variables for these beliefs.  相似文献   
548.
549.
Gender differences in susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
We examined data from a community survey of trauma exposure and DSM-IV PTSD in Winnipeg, Canada to explore factors associated with the higher rate of PTSD in women than men. Women were found to be at significantly increased risk for PTSD following exposure to serious trauma (odds ratio approximately 5), even when sexual trauma--which predominates in women--was excluded (odds ratio approximately 3). Adjusting for gender differences in the number of lifetime traumata, or in the likelihood of the trauma being associated with particular reactions to or consequences of the event (i.e. thinking that one would be killed or seriously injured; sustaining a serious physical injury; seeing someone else seriously injured or killed) did not result in a lessening of the PTSD risk in women. Women were found to be at increased risk for PTSD following nonsexual assaultive violence (e.g. mugging or other physical attack) but not following non-assaultive trauma (e.g. fire, witnessing injury to others). Understanding the basis of (and parameters for) this increased susceptibility to PTSD in women compared to men following trauma exposure should be a priority for future traumatic stress research.  相似文献   
550.
Three experiments investigated children's understanding of inference as a source of knowledge. Children observed a puppet make a statement about the color of one of two hidden toys after the puppet (a) looked directly at the toy (looking), (b) looked at the other toy (inference), or (c) looked at neither toy (guessing). Most 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds did not rate the puppet as being more certain of the toy's color after the puppet looked directly at it or inferred its color than they did after the puppet guessed its color. Most 8 and 9-year-olds distinguished inference and looking from guessing. The tendency to explain the puppet's knowledge by referring to inference increased with age. Children who referred to inference in their explanations were more likely to judge deductive inference as more certain than guessing.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号