全文获取类型
收费全文 | 259篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
266篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 19篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 2篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有266条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Studia Logica - We establish a duality between the category of involutive bisemilattices and the category of semilattice inverse systems of Stone spaces, using Stone duality from one side and the... 相似文献
52.
Stefano Carta 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2024,69(2):174-194
In his book Aion, Jung describes something like a quasi-Hegelian progressive historical realization of the Self in a perspective similar to Fernand Braudel’s longue durée history. This article deals with a similar perspective, as it tries to focus on what we may call a “cultural complex” yet within its unfolding in historical time and belonging not to just one specific cultural group, but to a large cultural basin, which we may indicate as the “West”. This complex marks the birth and development of modernity. The depth, pervasiveness and duration of this cultural complex permeates the lives and psychologies of all of those that are part of it. Therefore, every analytical project must take into account the underlying emotional, epistemic and social field within which this complex constellates. One of the main features of this (trans)cultural complex, strictly connected with the progressive fragmentation of the self and the transformation of the numinosum with its meaning-giving force, is paranoia. This article analyses the historical and cultural features that produce paranoia and fragmentation and determine paranoid symptoms and attitudes. 相似文献
53.
Brian S. Connelly Ronald A. Warren Hyunji Kim Stefano I. Di Domenico 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》2016,24(4):362-367
This article presents large‐sample developmental and validation research for a set of research scales of an existing 360‐degree personality measure, the LMAP 360 (Leadership Multi‐rater Assessment of Personality). In Study 1 (N = 1,771), we identified 6 broad domains underlying LMAP item clusters: Neuroticism, Dominance, Enthusiasm, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Scales measuring these broad domains and their constituent facets showed strong internal consistency, inter‐rater reliability, and self‐informant correlations. In Study 2 (N = 729 and N = 694), we examined LMAP research scales’ convergent and discriminant validity against three well‐validated personality inventories (Goldberg's adjectives, the Big Five Inventory, and the Big Five Aspects Scales) and one measure of cognitive ability (the International Cognitive Ability Resource). LMAP research scales correlated strongly with corresponding scales from other inventories and were distinct from cognitive ability. 相似文献
54.
55.
Chiara Milanese Stefano Corte Luca Salvetti Valentina Cavedon Tiziano Agostini 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(4):365-376
Performance errors drive motor learning for many tasks. The authors' aim was to determine which of two strategies, method of amplification of error (MAE) or direct instruction (DI), would be more beneficial for error correction during a full golfing swing with a driver. Thirty-four golfers were randomly assigned to one of three training conditions (MAE, DI, and control). Participants were tested in a practice session in which each golfer performed 7 pretraining trials, 6 training-intervention trials, and 7 posttraining trials; and a retention test after 1 week. An optoeletronic motion capture system was used to measure the kinematic parameters of each golfer's performance. Results showed that MAE is an effective strategy for correcting the technical errors leading to a rapid improvement in performance. These findings could have practical implications for sport psychology and physical education because, while practice is obviously necessary for improving learning, the efficacy of the learning process is essential in enhancing learners' motivation and sport enjoyment. 相似文献
56.
Simeoni S Biselli R D'Amelio R Rocca B Lattanzio S Mucci L Davì G Patacchioli FR 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2011,14(3):282-289
Few studies have assessed the effects of stress on in vivo platelet activation. In the present study, hypobaric hypoxia induced by rapid decompression during high-altitude simulated flight in a hypobaric chamber was used to evaluate the effects of environmental stress on salivary cortisol and urinary thromboxane metabolite (TXM) excretion, a noninvasive marker of in vivo platelet function. Twenty-one male aviators (mean ± SD age = 36 ± 7 years) experiencing hypoxia by removing their oxygen mask for 4-5 min during a simulated flight to 25,000 ft (7,620 m; pO(2) = 59.17 mmHg) and a matched control group of thirteen flying instructors wearing oxygen masks during the challenge, were studied. Hypobaric hypoxia induced a transient significant increase (P < 0.001) in the aviators' salivary cortisol concentration; the overall pattern of diurnal cortisol fluctuation was maintained in both groups. Urinary TXM showed a significant ~30% reduction (P < 0.01) after the chamber session in aviators exposed to hypobaric hypoxia, but not in controls. A significant inverse correlation was found between salivary cortisol and urinary TXM in aviators (r = - 0.64, P = 0.0015). Salivary cortisol was a significant predictor (P < 0.001) for urinary TXM concentrations in aviators. In conclusion, here we observed that an acute stress-induced salivary cortisol increase was associated with reduced urinary thromboxane biosynthesis, providing the first indirect evidence for an inhibitory effect of acute stress on in vivo platelet function. 相似文献
57.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
58.
The System Usability Scale (SUS), developed by Brooke (Usability evaluation in industry, Taylor & Francis, London, pp 189–194,
1996), had a great success among usability practitioners since it is a quick and easy to use measure for collecting users’ usability
evaluation of a system. Recently, Lewis and Sauro (Proceedings of the human computer interaction international conference
(HCII 2009), San Diego CA, USA, 2009) have proposed a two-factor structure—Usability (8 items) and Learnability (2 items)—suggesting that practitioners might
take advantage of these new factors to extract additional information from SUS data. In order to verify the dimensionality
in the SUS’ two-component structure, we estimated the parameters and tested with a structural equation model the SUS structure
on a sample of 196 university users. Our data indicated that both the unidimensional model and the two-factor model with uncorrelated
factors proposed by Lewis and Sauro (Proceedings of the human computer interaction international conference (HCII 2009), San
Diego CA, USA, 2009) had a not satisfactory fit to the data. We thus released the hypothesis that Usability and Learnability are independent
components of SUS ratings and tested a less restrictive model with correlated factors. This model not only yielded a good
fit to the data, but it was also significantly more appropriate to represent the structure of SUS ratings.
相似文献
Simone BorsciEmail: Email: |
59.
The value of λ is one of the main issues debated in international usability studies. The debate is centred on the deficiencies
of the mathematical return on investment model (ROI model) of Nielsen and Landauer (1993). The ROI model is discussed in order
to identify the base of another model that, respecting Nielsen and Landauer’s one, tries to consider a large number of variables
for the estimation of the number of evaluators needed for an interface. Using the bootstrap model (Efron 1979), we can take into account: (a) the interface properties, as the properties at zero condition of evaluation and (b) the probability
that the population discovery behaviour is represented by all the possible discovery behaviours of a sample. Our alternative
model, named Bootstrap Discovery Behaviour (BDB), provides an alternative estimation of the number of experts and users needed
for a usability evaluation. Two experimental groups of users and experts are involved in the evaluation of a website (). Applying the BDB model to the problems identified by the two groups, we found that 13 experts and 20 users are needed to
identify 80% of usability problems, instead of 6 experts and 7 users required according to the estimation of the discovery
likelihood provided by the ROI model. The consequence of the difference between the results of those models is that in following
the BDB the costs of usability evaluation increase, although this is justified considering that the results obtained have
the best probability of representing the entire population of experts and users. 相似文献
60.
Paul M. Bronstein Stefano Parmigiani Patrizia Torricelli Paul F. Brain 《Aggressive behavior》1988,14(5):363-370
Males of two species of freshwater fish (convict cichlids and Italian freshwater gobies) fought with conspecifics and subsequently remained near the visual and spatial cues that had been paired with those social encounters. These data show that postfight preference for combat sites is a phenomenon of some generality among teleosts. The likely role of learning in establishing postfight preferences for the locales of aggression is discussed. 相似文献