全文获取类型
收费全文 | 652篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
662篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 87篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 29篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 40篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有662条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Dufau S Grainger J Ziegler JC 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2012,38(4):1117-1128
We describe a leaky competing accumulator (LCA) model of the lexical decision task that can be used as a response/decision module for any computational model of word recognition. The LCA model uses evidence for a word, operationalized as some measure of lexical activity, as input to the YES decision node. Input to the NO decision node is simply a constant value minus evidence for a word. In this way, evidence for a nonword is a function of time from stimulus onset (as in standard deadline models) modulated by lexical activity via the competitive dynamics of the LCA. We propose a simple mechanism for determining the value of this constant online during the first trials of a lexical decision experiment, such that the model can rapidly optimize speed and accuracy in discriminating words from nonwords. Further optimization is achieved via trial-by-trial adjustments in response criteria as a function of task demands and list context. We show that the LCA model can simulate mean response times and response distributions for correct and incorrect YES and NO decisions for a number of benchmark experiments that have been shown to be fatal for deadline models of lexical decision. Finally, using lexical activity calculated by a computational model of word recognition as input to the LCA decision module, we provide the first item-level simulation of both word and nonword responses in a large-scale database. 相似文献
72.
73.
74.
Francis Gingras Daniel Fiset Marie-Pier Plouffe-Demers Andréa Deschênes Stéphanie Cormier Hélène Forget Caroline Blais 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2023,114(3):621-637
Pain experienced by Black individuals is systematically underestimated, and recent studies have shown that part of this bias is rooted in perceptual factors. We used Reverse Correlation to estimate visual representations of the pain expression in Black and White faces, in participants originating from both Western and African countries. Groups of raters were then asked to evaluate the presence of pain and other emotions in these representations. A second group of White raters then evaluated those same representations placed over a neutral background face (50% White; 50% Black). Image-based analyses show significant effects of culture and face ethnicity, but no interaction between the two factors. Western representations were more likely to be judged as expressing pain than African representations. For both cultural groups, raters also perceived more pain in White face representations than in Black face representations. However, when changing the background stimulus to the neutral background face, this effect of face ethnic profile disappeared. Overall, these results suggest that individuals have different expectations of how pain is expressed by Black and White individuals, and that cultural factors may explain a part of this phenomenon 相似文献
75.
Manon Marquet Pierre Missotten Benoit Dardenne Stéphane Adam 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2019,26(1):121-143
This study examined whether the effects of stereotype threat on memory and subjective age were moderated by positive age stereotypes and self-perceptions of aging among older adults. Perceived threat as a mechanism underlying these effects was also explored. Results showed that stereotype threat (high vs. low threat) did not affect the dependent variables. Moreover, self-perceptions of aging did not moderate the effect of stereotype threat on the dependent variables. However, for people with more positive age stereotypes, older people under highthreat perceived more threat than people under low threat. This could be explained by an effect of age stereotypes in the high-threat group: the more positive age stereotypes held by participants, the more they perceived threat, which in turn decreased their memory performance and made them feel mentally older. We hypothesized that age group identity is stronger in people with more positive age stereotypes, which increase perceived threat. 相似文献
76.
We compared rates of academic responses and problem behavior during mathematics with distributed and accumulated reinforcer arrangements for 3 students with Attention‐Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder who engaged in chronic, severe problem behavior. All 3 students engaged in more academic responding and less problem behavior when reinforcers accumulated throughout the session, relative to conditions in which reinforcers were distributed throughout the session or withheld completely. We then conducted concurrent‐chain analyses to evaluate student preference for the reinforcer arrangements. Two students preferred distributed reinforcers, even though this arrangement continued to produce problem behavior. One student preferred accumulated reinforcers. Our data replicate previous findings regarding the efficacy of accumulated‐reinforcer arrangements, but suggest that students do not always prefer the most efficacious reinforcer arrangement. 相似文献
77.
Approximately 2.5 million children in the European Union and the United States have incarcerated parents, the vast majority of which are fathers. Three experiments modeled on real legal cases (total N = 881) investigated how parent gender affects decisions regarding contact between incarcerated parents and their children. Results showed that measures facilitating relationship maintenance in relevant domains (sentence length, visitation rights, and alleviating postsentencing conditions) were supported less when they involved a father despite identical prior information about the legal case. Mediation analyses suggest two distinct processes explaining these disparities: participants' crime‐related attributions, and their stereotypical expectations about the different familial roles of mothers and fathers. Practical implications of these findings and directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
78.
Joël Billieux Alexandre Heeren Lucien Rochat Pierre Maurage Sophie Bayard Romain Bet Chrystel Besche-Richard Gaëlle Challet-Bouju Arnaud Carré Gaëtan Devos Maèva Flayelle Fabien Gierski Marie Grall-Bronnec Laurence Kern Yasser Khazaal Christophe Lançon Séverine Lannoy George A. Michael Stéphane Raffard Lucia Romo Martial Van der Linden Aline Wéry Natale Canale Daniel L. King Adriano Schimmenti Stéphanie Baggio 《Journal of personality》2021,89(6):1252-1262
79.
80.
William E. Hartmann Denise M. St. Arnault Joseph P. Gone 《American journal of community psychology》2018,61(1-2):62-75
Community psychology (CP) abandoned the clinic and disengaged from movements for community mental health (CMH) to escape clinical convention and pursue growing aspirations as an independent field of context‐oriented, community‐engaged, and values‐driven research and action. In doing so, however, CP positioned itself on the sidelines of influential contemporary movements that promote potentially harmful, reductionist biomedical narratives in mental health. We advocate for a return to the clinic—the seat of institutional power in mental health—using critical clinic‐based inquiry to open sites for clinical‐community dialogue that can instigate transformative change locally and nationally. To inform such works within the collaborative and emancipatory traditions of CP, we detail a recently completed clinical ethnography and offer “lessons learned” regarding challenges likely to re‐emerge in similar efforts. Conducted with an urban American Indian community behavioral health clinic, this ethnography examined how culture and culture concepts (e.g., cultural competence) shaped clinical practice with socio‐political implications for American Indian peoples and the pursuit of transformative change in CMH. Lessons learned identify exceptional clinicians versed in ecological thinking and contextualist discourses of human suffering as ideal partners for this work; encourage intense contextualization and constraining critique to areas of mutual interest; and support relational approaches to clinic collaborations. 相似文献