全文获取类型
收费全文 | 84318篇 |
免费 | 1512篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
85837篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 995篇 |
2019年 | 1196篇 |
2018年 | 1662篇 |
2017年 | 1649篇 |
2016年 | 1782篇 |
2015年 | 1268篇 |
2014年 | 1517篇 |
2013年 | 6965篇 |
2012年 | 2788篇 |
2011年 | 2813篇 |
2010年 | 1701篇 |
2009年 | 1750篇 |
2008年 | 2396篇 |
2007年 | 2382篇 |
2006年 | 2146篇 |
2005年 | 1815篇 |
2004年 | 1846篇 |
2003年 | 1737篇 |
2002年 | 1695篇 |
2001年 | 2646篇 |
2000年 | 2546篇 |
1999年 | 1890篇 |
1998年 | 890篇 |
1997年 | 802篇 |
1996年 | 846篇 |
1995年 | 811篇 |
1994年 | 766篇 |
1993年 | 756篇 |
1992年 | 1562篇 |
1991年 | 1442篇 |
1990年 | 1433篇 |
1989年 | 1353篇 |
1988年 | 1354篇 |
1987年 | 1243篇 |
1986年 | 1230篇 |
1985年 | 1327篇 |
1984年 | 1083篇 |
1983年 | 976篇 |
1982年 | 754篇 |
1979年 | 1137篇 |
1978年 | 833篇 |
1975年 | 905篇 |
1974年 | 1052篇 |
1973年 | 1095篇 |
1972年 | 954篇 |
1971年 | 865篇 |
1969年 | 764篇 |
1968年 | 964篇 |
1967年 | 850篇 |
1966年 | 819篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
831.
Tolbutamide has previously been shown to amplify the pressor effects of “exogenous” catecholamines in conscious dogs, possibly due to sensitization of the α1-adrenoreceptor-mediated vasoconstriction. The objective of this study was to examine if tolbutamide also amplifies the pressor effects of “endogenous” catecholamines released during psychological stress (classical Pavlovian aversive conditioning). Experiments were conducted in β-adrenoreceptor-blocked (propranolol, 1 mg/kg, i.v.) conscious dogs (n=4) trained in classical aversive conditioning. Conditioning was accomplished by following a tone (CS+) with a 1/2 second shock; another tone (CS-) was not followed by any shock and served as control. With saline pretreatment, aversive conditioning (i.e., CS+) increased mean arterial pressure (MAP) only by approximately 4.7% when compared to CS-, whereas with tolbutamide (45 mg/kg, i.v.) pretreatment, the increase in MAP induced by CS+ beyond what was induced by the CS- (approximately 6.2%) was significantly (p<0.05) larger than that with saline pretreatment. In isolated canine femoral arterial segments (n=4), the vasoconstrictor effect of phenylephrine (an α1-agonist) at 5×10?6M (which was the EC50 value) was amplified by 2×10?2M of tolbutamide from 54.0±2.0% to 66.9±2.1%. In conclusion, tolbutamide amplifies the pressor effects of “endogenous” catecholamines in conscious dogs, possibly by sensitization of the α1-adrenoreceptor-mediated vasoconstriction. This mechanism of action is novel and has not been reported with other agents. 相似文献
832.
Sjoerd C. de Vries Astrid M. L. Kappers Jan J. Koenderink 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1993,53(1):71-80
We used quadratic shapes in several psychophysical shape-from-stereo tasks. The shapes were elegantly represented in a 2-D parameter space by the scale-independentshape index and the scale-dependentcurvedness. Using random-dot stereograms to depict the surfaces, we found that the shape of hyperbolic surfaces is slightly more difficult to recognize than the shape of elliptic surfaces. We found that curvedness (and indirectly, scale) has little or no influence on shape recognition. 相似文献
833.
Georgije Lukatela Katerina Lukatela M. T. Turvey 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1993,53(5):461-466
If the phonological codes of visually presented words are assembled-rapidly and automatically for use in lexical access, then words that sound alike should induce similar activity within the internal lexicon.Towed is homophonous with TOAD, which is semantically related tofrog, andbeach is homophonous withbeech, which is semantically related totree. Stimuli such as these were used in a priming-of-namingtask, in which words homophonous with associates of the target words preceded the targets at an onset asynchrony of 100 msec. Relative to spelling controls (trod, bench), the low-frequencytowed and the high-frequencybeach speeded up the naming offrog andtree, respectively, to the same degree. This result was discussed in relation to the accumulating evidence for the primacy of phonological constraints in visual lexical access. nt]mis|This research was supported in part by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grants HD-08945 and HD-0 1994 to the first author and Haskins Laboratories, respectively. 相似文献
834.
LEON SLOMAN M.D. F.R.C.P.. STEPHEN SPRINGER Ph.D. C.Psych. MARY L.S. VACHON R.N. Ph.D. 《Family process》1993,32(2):171-183
When a deaf child is born to hearing parents, a grieving process is initiated in the parents. Unresolved grieving over their child's deafness often makes it difficult for hearing parents to accept the importance of signing, thus increasing the child's problems — further source of grief for these parents. Clinical illustrations are provided of (1) the reciprocal relationship between disruption of the mourning process and disturbance of communication between family members, and (2) the transmission of the dysfunctional relationship between hearing parents and deaf children to the subsequent relationship between the deaf children, when they reach adulthood, and their hearing children. 相似文献
835.
The relationship between major mental illness and the family is complex. This article describes a model that recognizes biological, individual, and familial aspects of illness, and the interaction and reciprocal effects of these different levels. This model is illustrated using the example of bipolar illness, with particular emphasis on the effects of the illness on the family. Many characteristics previously noted in bipolar families can be understood as consequences, rather than causes, of the illness. A framework for integrated treatment is also described. 相似文献
836.
837.
838.
839.
The motor system may use internal predictive models of the motor apparatus to achieve better control than would be possible by negative feedback. Several theories have proposed that the cerebellum may form these predictive representations. In this article, we review these theories and try to unify them by reference to an engineering control model known as a Smith Predictor. We suggest that the cerebellum forms two types of internal model. One model is a forward predictive model of the motor apparatus (e.g., limb and muscle), providing a rapid prediction of the sensory consequences of each movement. The second model is of the time delays in the control loop (due to receptor and effector delays, axonal conductances, and cognitive processing delays). This model delays a copy of the rapid prediction so that it can be compared in temporal register with actual sensory feedback from the movement. The result of this comparison is used both to correct for errors in performance and as a training signal to learn the first model. We discuss evidence that the cerebellum could form both of these models and suggest that the cerebellum may hold at least two separate Smith Predictors. One, in the lateral cerebellum, would predict the movement outcome in visual, egocentric, or peripersonal coordinates. Another, in the intermediate cerebellum, would predict the consequences in motor coordinates. Generalization of the Smith Predictor theory is discussed in light of cerebellar involvement in nonmotor control systems, including autonomic functions and cognition. 相似文献
840.
Intentions are an important concept in Artificial Intelligence and Cognitive Science. We present a formal theory of intentions and beliefs based on Discourse Representation Theory that captures many of their important logical properties. Unlike possible worlds approaches, this theory does not assume that agents are perfect reasoners, and gives a realistic view of their internal architecture; unlike most representational approaches, it has anobjective semantics, and does not rely on anad hoc labeling of the internal states of agents. We describe a minimal logic for intentions and beliefs that is sound and complete relative to our semantics. We discuss several additional axioms, and the constraints on the models that validate them. 相似文献