全文获取类型
收费全文 | 27157篇 |
免费 | 283篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
27442篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 56篇 |
2020年 | 110篇 |
2019年 | 125篇 |
2018年 | 3596篇 |
2017年 | 2956篇 |
2016年 | 2385篇 |
2015年 | 337篇 |
2014年 | 246篇 |
2013年 | 926篇 |
2012年 | 832篇 |
2011年 | 2633篇 |
2010年 | 2652篇 |
2009年 | 1651篇 |
2008年 | 1948篇 |
2007年 | 2408篇 |
2006年 | 282篇 |
2005年 | 471篇 |
2004年 | 411篇 |
2003年 | 338篇 |
2002年 | 290篇 |
2001年 | 136篇 |
2000年 | 149篇 |
1999年 | 127篇 |
1998年 | 143篇 |
1997年 | 99篇 |
1996年 | 91篇 |
1995年 | 95篇 |
1994年 | 80篇 |
1993年 | 70篇 |
1992年 | 71篇 |
1991年 | 71篇 |
1990年 | 64篇 |
1989年 | 65篇 |
1988年 | 60篇 |
1987年 | 52篇 |
1986年 | 55篇 |
1985年 | 75篇 |
1984年 | 73篇 |
1983年 | 68篇 |
1982年 | 60篇 |
1981年 | 62篇 |
1980年 | 63篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 63篇 |
1977年 | 57篇 |
1976年 | 64篇 |
1975年 | 58篇 |
1974年 | 54篇 |
1973年 | 43篇 |
1972年 | 37篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
22.
Speech nonfluency in response to questions about the marital relationship was used to assess anxiety. Subjects were 31 husbands and 31 wives, all white, college educated, from middle- to lower-middle-class families, and ranging from 20 to 30 years of age. Three types of nonfluencies were coded: filled pauses, unfilled pauses, and repetitions. Speech-disturbance ratios were computed by dividing the sum of speech nonfluencies by the total words spoken. The results support the notion that some issues within marriage are more sensitive and/or problematic than others, and that, in an interview situation, gender interacts with question content in the production of nonfluencies. 相似文献
23.
24.
Paul K. Moser 《Erkenntnis》1988,28(2):231-251
Epistemological probability is the kind of probability relative to a body of evidence. Many philosophers, including Henry Kyburg and Roderick Chisholm, hold that all epistemological probabilities reflect a relation between an evidential body of propositions and other propositions. But this article argues that some epistemological probabilities for empirical propositions must be relative to non-propositional evidence, specifically the contents of non-propositional perceptual states. In doing so, the article distinguishes between internalism and externalism regarding epistemological probability, and argues for a version of awareness internalism. The article draws three main concluding lessons. First, epistemological probability is not to be identified with the sort of objective, experience-independent probability that is familiar from statistical and propensity interpretations of probability. Second, it is doubtful that epistemological probability is measurable, in any useful way, by real numbers, even if it admits of comparative assessments. Third, contrary to the familiar claim of C. I. Lewis, epistemological probability should not be viewed as requiring a basis of certainty. 相似文献
25.
26.
A method for analyzing test item responses is proposed to examine differential item functioning (DIF) in multiple-choice items through a combination of the usual notion of DIF, for correct/incorrect responses and information about DIF contained in each of the alternatives. The proposed method uses incomplete latent class models to examine whether DIF is caused by the attractiveness of the alternatives, difficulty of the item, or both. DIF with respect to either known or unknown subgroups can be tested by a likelihood ratio test that is asymptotically distributed as a chi-square random variable. 相似文献
27.
28.
Paul Sparks Duncan Hedderley Richard Shepherd 《European journal of social psychology》1992,22(1):55-71
The study reported here takes its lead from the literatures which emphasize the importance of attitude variability and the role of perceived control over action. Within-person variability and perceptions of control are investigated in the context of people's attitudes towards the consumption of two common foods. The role of attitude ambivalence is also examined. The findings indicate that higher attitude variability is associated with weaker relationships between the components of the theory of reasoned action and that attitude variability is negatively related to perceived control. Moreover, perceived control is shown to be related to different sorts of control problem for different behaviours. It is advocated that a more in-depth assessment of attitude variability and the perceived control construct is merited and that recent calls for more serious examination of attitude ambivalence are well-founded. 相似文献
29.
Coby Gerlsma Ineke Mosterman Sytske Buwalda Paul M. G. Emmelkamp 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1992,14(4):343-361
The relationship between memories of childhood experiences (e.g., adverse parenting) and adult depression often found raises questions of interpretation. On the one hand, both laboratory studies and clinicians' experiences suggest that subjects in a depressed mood frequently show a negative bias in perceptions and memories. Negative childhood memories in depressed persons might, therefore, be interpreted as epiphenomena of depressed mood instead of etiological factors. On the other hand, memories of childhood experiences seem remarkably stable across changes in depressed mood, especially when memories are elicited by means of standardized questionnaires. In the mood and memory literature several explanations for this stability are offered. For one thing, highly structured cues to elicit memories (such as in questionnaires) are hypothesized to be less susceptible to mood bias than unstructured memory cues (such as in free recall procedures). On the other hand, resource allocation theorists suggest that childhood memories, being well established and rehearsed, are relatively impervious to mood bias no matter how they are elicited. In this study we examined whether different methods of eliciting childhood memories (i.e., free recall and questionnaire-cued) are differentially susceptible to mood bias. To this aim, we used a mood induction procedure to induce depressed, neutral, and elated mood and assessed childhood memories both before and after the mood induction using both questionnaires and free recall to elicit memories. Results suggested that memories elicited by means of free recall as well as by means of questionnaire-cued recall were susceptible to depressed and elated mood bias. The implications for research addressing the link between childhood experiences and depression are discussed. 相似文献
30.
Paul A. M. van Lange 《欧洲人格杂志》1992,6(5):371-379
Kelley and Stahelski's (1970) triangle hypothesis states that competitors hold homogeneous views of others by assuming that most others are competitive, whereas cooperators or pro-social people hold more heterogeneous views by assuming that others are either cooperative or competitive. To evaluate the triangle hypothesis, this study examines differences between pro-socials, individualists, and competitors not only in their expectations about others' choice behaviour, but also in the confidence with which such expectations are held. It was found that pro-social subjects expected more cooperation than individualists and competitors. More importantly, as predicted on the basis of the triangle hypothesis, pro-socials were less confident about their expectations than competitors, with individualists holding intermediate levels of confidence. 相似文献