首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   390篇
  免费   45篇
  国内免费   87篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   11篇
  2021年   11篇
  2020年   19篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   17篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   37篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   25篇
  2009年   27篇
  2008年   34篇
  2007年   24篇
  2006年   30篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   19篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   1篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
排序方式: 共有522条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Infants' reasoning about others' false perceptions   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Prior research suggests that children younger than age 3 or 4 do not understand that an agent may be deceived by an object's misleading appearance. The authors asked whether 14.5-month-olds would give evidence in a violation-of-expectation task that they understand that agents may form false perceptions. Infants first watched events in which an agent faced a stuffed skunk and a doll with blue pigtails; the agent consistently reached for the doll, suggesting that she preferred it over the skunk. Next, while the agent was absent, the doll was hidden in a plain box, and the skunk was hidden in a box with a tuft of blue hair protruding from under its lid. Infants expected the agent to be misled by the tuft's resemblance to the doll's hair and to falsely perceive it as belonging to the doll. These and other results indicate that 14.5-month-old infants can already reason about agents' false perceptions.  相似文献   
82.
In this study, the authors examined affective experiences of dual-earner couples. More specifically, the authors explored how momentary moods can spill over between work and family and cross over from one spouse to another. Fifty couples used their cell phones to provide reports of their momentary moods over 8 consecutive days. Results show significant spillover and crossover effects for both positive and negative moods. Work orientation moderated negative mood spillover from work to home, and the presence of children in the family decreased negative mood crossover between spouses. Crossover was observed when spouses were physically together and when the time interval between the spouses' reports was short. With this study, the authors contribute to the work and family research by examining the nature of mood transfers among dual-earner couples, including the direction, valence, and moderators of these transfers across work and family domains. The authors also contribute to the event sampling methodology by introducing a new method of using cell phones to collect momentary data.  相似文献   
83.
宋婕 《现代哲学》2006,2(3):14-24
传统中国蕴含着两种“革命”思想的维度:一是圣人救世的“汤武革命”,一是基于压迫而进行反抗的农民起义。这两个维度对于既系统地接受过中国传统的规范性文化教育,同时又深受非规范性文化熏染的毛泽东来说都产生了极大的影响,并形成了他“圣人救世”与“压迫-反抗”的两种“革命”逻辑。这两种具有不同文化气质的“革命”逻辑在毛泽东思想中得以结合的根本基础是他尚变求动的宇宙观和人生观。  相似文献   
84.
宋立宏 《学海》2006,10(1):13-19
犹太人只要交纳必需的赋税,只要不威胁国家安全,罗马政府就允许他们按照其祖先的律法生活。犹太人虽然不拥有完全的自治权,但有权裁决内部纠纷,其宗教自由也能够得到罗马政权的保障和宽容。自恺撒为这种政策奠定恰当的法律形式以来,它在基督教获得全面胜利以前具有很强的稳定性。由于有关犹太人的文献在罗马世界的地方文献中保存得最好,考察罗马的犹太政策为我们了解罗马的宗教政策和民族政策提供了独特的视角。  相似文献   
85.
成败情境下不同目标取向学生内在动机特点   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
以213名小学生为被试,设置成功和失败情境,考察三种成就目标取向学生内在动机的差异。结果发现:两种情境下,掌握目标的学生均有最高的内在动机;失败情境下,成绩接近目标学生的内在动机显著高于成绩回避目标的学生,但成功情境下,两种目标取向学生的内在动机没有显著差异;掌握目标学生在两种情境下的内在动机无显著差异;成绩接近和成绩回避目标学生在失败情境下的内在动机显著低于成功情境下的内在动机。  相似文献   
86.
在大鼠条件性饮水反应的建立和巩固过程中,在每天训练之前,通过预先与记录电极一起埋植在海马齿状回的注药管,微量注射去甲肾上腺素(NE),观察其对海马齿状回的习得性长时程突触增强的影响。结果表明:NE能增强突触效应;训练作业前注NE易化了习得性长时程突触增强的形成,但对其巩固无影响;同时相应地易化了动物条件反应的建立。提示NE参与海马齿状回的习得性长时程增强的形成。本文对其可能的作用机制进行了讨论。  相似文献   
87.
第三次分配是构建和谐社会的重要途径   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
宋林飞 《学海》2007,(3):68-72
第三次分配是克服第一次分配的弊端,以及弥补第二次分配不足的重要社会分配方式。发展慈善事业,减少贫困与缩小贫富差距,是构建社会主义和谐社会的重要途径。通过加强社会调节,促使更多的个人和群体参与第三次分配。探索符合中国国情的慈善事业发展模式,需要积极培育与发展慈善组织,建立现代财富伦理,推进慈善文化的发育与普及,增强企业家慈善捐款的热情,提高慈善基金会的社会公信度,鼓励志愿者的慈善义工,大力推进慈善理论研究与创新。  相似文献   
88.
The present research examined whether 9.5-month-old infants can attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action on objects, and can then use this disposition to predict which of two new objects - one that can be used to perform the action and one that cannot - the agent is likely to reach for next. The infants first received familiarization trials in which they watched an agent slide either three (Experiments 1 and 3) or six (Experiment 2) different objects forward and backward on an apparatus floor. During test, the infants saw two new identical objects placed side by side: one stood inside a short frame that left little room for sliding, and the other stood inside a longer frame that left ample room for sliding. The infants who saw the agent slide six different objects attributed to her a disposition to slide objects: they expected her to select the "slidable" as opposed to the "unslidable" test object, and they looked reliably longer when she did not. In contrast, the infants who saw the agent slide only three different objects looked about equally when she selected either test object. These results add to recent evidence that infants in the first year of life can attribute dispositions to agents, and can use these dispositions to help predict agents' actions in new contexts.  相似文献   
89.
Individuals often rely on their affect for information. However, positive or negative affect may not be informative for individuals who are more prone to feel affect of this particular valence in general, unless they chronically monitor their internal states. A series of studies demonstrates that differences in individuals’ internal versus external focus of attention and in trait affective valence associated with their approach versus avoidance motivation determine reliance on affect as information. Specifically, our findings show that while positive affect is used as an input in product satisfaction judgments regardless of individuals’ approach versus avoidance motivation, negative affect is used only by individuals with a predominant approach motivation.  相似文献   
90.
Studies into interactions between explicit and implicit motor sequence learning have yielded mixed results. Some of these discrepancies have been attributed to difficulties in isolating implicit learning. In the present study, the effect of explicit knowledge on implicit learning was investigated using a modified version of the Alternating Serial Response Time (ASRT) task, a probabilistic sequence learning paradigm that yields continuous and relatively pure measures of implicit learning. Results revealed that implicit learning occurred to the same extent, whether or not subjects had explicit knowledge. Some evidence, however, indicated that explicit knowledge could interfere with the expression of implicit learning early in training. In addition, there were dissociations between learning measures, in that reaction time and accuracy were differentially affected by explicit knowledge. These findings indicate that implicit sequence learning occurs independently of explicit knowledge, and help to explain previous discrepant findings.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号