全文获取类型
收费全文 | 166篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
专业分类
183篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 4篇 |
2013年 | 10篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1955年 | 1篇 |
1954年 | 2篇 |
1952年 | 1篇 |
1951年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有183条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Jones Kimberley A. Freijah Isabella Carey Lindsay Carleton R. Nicholas Devenish-Meares Peter Dell Lisa Rodrigues Sara Madden Kelsey Johnson Lucinda Hosseiny Fardous Phelps Andrea J. 《Journal of religion and health》2022,61(2):1051-1094
Journal of Religion and Health - The aim of this research was to describe the evidence examining the approaches taken by mental health providers (MHPs) and chaplains to address symptoms related to... 相似文献
32.
The present experiments examined a revised version of the Eriksen and Yeh model of attentional allocation during visual search. The results confirmed the assumption of the model that performance represents a weighted combination of focused- and distributed-attention trials, although Ss relied on focused attention more than was predicted. Consistent with the model, predictions on the basis of the assumption of a terminating search fit the data better than predictions on the basis of an exhaustive search. The effects of varying cue validity favored an interpretation of focused attention in terms of a processing gradient rather than a zoom lens. Although the allocation of attention across trials was similar for young and older adults, there was an age-related increase in the time required to allocate attention within individual trials. 相似文献
33.
34.
Dr. Myron C. Madden Ph.D. 《Pastoral Psychology》1976,25(1):34-38
Closeness, warmth, perhaps touching, and some understanding are characteristic of interpersonal relationships. However, there are dangers of intrusion and insensitivity when persons invite pastors to become intimate. Superficial and false intimacy may substitute for closeness. Pastoral care should transcend the primitive holy man role, which is cold because the holy is desexualized. Intimacy is a closeness between persons in areas we tend to guard as personal.He is the author ofThe Power to Bless andRaise the Dead. 相似文献
35.
Previous research has suggested that, in visual-search tasks, the comparison between target and display items does not require attentional capacity. In the present experiment we used a secondary-task paradigm to distinguish the amount and duration of the attentional demands of visual search. The subjects performed visual search (the primary task) and tone detection (the secondary task) concurrently over the course of five experimental sessions (1,440 trials). For each subject, target-response mapping was either consistent or varied for Days 1-5. The results indicate that the amount of attentional demand, as reflected in secondary-task performance, increased as a function of display size in the search task. Switching from consistent to varied mapping in a sixth experimental session increased both the amount and the duration of the attentional demands of the search. The present results support models of visual-search performance in which the comparison of target and display items requires attentional capacity. 相似文献
36.
37.
Human Sensitivity To Concurrent Schedules Of Reinforcement: Effects Of Observing Schedule-correlated Stimuli 下载免费PDF全文
The determinants of human sensitivity to concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules of reinforcement have been difficult to identify, in part because of procedural differences separating published experiments. This experiment investigated vigilance to stimuli correlated with concurrent schedules. Across phases, 3 college students were provided with either no schedule-correlated stimuli, an observing response that provided brief access to the stimuli, or a contingency that required the subject to identify the stimulus correlated with the source of each obtained reinforcer. Sensitivity, as quantified by the generalized matching equation, was low when no stimuli were available. When the stimuli were response contingent, 1 subject observed them, and her behavior became more sensitive to the distribution of reinforcers across the concurrent schedules. When the procedure required discrimination of the stimulus correlated with each reinforcer, the other 2 subjects also observed the stimuli, and their schedule sensitivity was increased as well. These results implicate procedural differences, rather than inherent behavioral differences, as the source of differences in sensitivity to schedules of reinforcement between humans and nonhumans. 相似文献
38.
Adult age differences in the mode of allocation of visual attention were investigated, using a visual search task with a circular display containing one target letter and seven distractor letters. In two experiments, a total of 56 younger adults (M = 20 years) and 56 older adults (M = 66 years) searched for a target appearing with equal probability at one of two cued locations. The first cue appeared 115 msec before display onset, and the second cue appeared with display onset; distance between the two cued locations was varied. Target identification performance indicated that attention was inertial, in that reaction time for second-cued targets was related either to the area of the portion of the visual field containing possible target locations or to the mean path length of a serial self-terminating search. There were no age-related decrements in the allocation of visual attention. 相似文献
39.
40.