全文获取类型
收费全文 | 19266篇 |
免费 | 378篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
19645篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 175篇 |
2019年 | 280篇 |
2018年 | 341篇 |
2017年 | 293篇 |
2016年 | 332篇 |
2015年 | 237篇 |
2014年 | 324篇 |
2013年 | 1556篇 |
2012年 | 536篇 |
2011年 | 580篇 |
2010年 | 371篇 |
2009年 | 370篇 |
2008年 | 492篇 |
2007年 | 532篇 |
2006年 | 507篇 |
2005年 | 412篇 |
2004年 | 444篇 |
2003年 | 385篇 |
2002年 | 385篇 |
2001年 | 562篇 |
2000年 | 625篇 |
1999年 | 415篇 |
1998年 | 231篇 |
1997年 | 187篇 |
1996年 | 191篇 |
1995年 | 179篇 |
1992年 | 318篇 |
1991年 | 338篇 |
1990年 | 337篇 |
1989年 | 318篇 |
1988年 | 311篇 |
1987年 | 265篇 |
1986年 | 267篇 |
1985年 | 284篇 |
1984年 | 219篇 |
1983年 | 231篇 |
1982年 | 178篇 |
1979年 | 271篇 |
1978年 | 232篇 |
1976年 | 175篇 |
1975年 | 233篇 |
1974年 | 263篇 |
1973年 | 292篇 |
1972年 | 237篇 |
1971年 | 231篇 |
1970年 | 186篇 |
1969年 | 212篇 |
1968年 | 269篇 |
1967年 | 264篇 |
1966年 | 223篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
C F Michaels 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(2):231-240
In choice reaction time, stimuli and responses in some combinations (e.g., based on spatial arrangement) are faster than in other combinations. To test whether motion toward a position yields faster responses at that position, a computer-generated square in front of one hand appeared to move either toward that hand or toward the other hand. Compatible responses (e.g., motion toward left hand/left response) were faster than incompatible responses, even when that opposed traditional positional compatibility. In Experiment 2, subjects responded to the same stimuli but with both hands left, right, or on the body midline. Medial responses were the fastest, showing that destination, rather than mere relative position, was a critical variable. It was suggested that spatial compatibility effects are not unique to position but apply to a variety of task situations, describable by J.J. Gibson's theory of affordances, in which he claims that one perceives the actions (e.g., catching) permitted in a situation. 相似文献
22.
23.
24.
F R D'Amato 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1988,102(2):146-151
Differences in reproduction as well as in behavior in the presence of females were evaluated according to dominant and subordinate male rank in albino mice, in the temporary absence of each male's antagonist. Dominant males reproduced more successfully than subordinate males. Subordinate males were generally inactive, except for displacement activities, during the first 15 min they were exposed to female partners. These findings suggest that mechanisms other than male-male interference or mating order may be operating or influencing behavior and reproductive results. 相似文献
25.
Counseling training programs have failed to differentiate between and among behavioral, cognitive, and unconsciousness dimesnsions. This omission has resulted in confusion and competition in many counseling curricula in that valuable programs are competing for space rather than being integrated systematically. This article proposes a multilevel model of intentionality which integrates the several aspects of the counselor-client relationship. In turn, this integration of behavioral, cognitive, and unconscious dimensions will have important applications in counselor daily practice. 相似文献
26.
F Busch 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》1992,40(4):1089-1115
Analysis of the unconscious ego resistances is one of those clinical concepts more honored in the breach than in the observance. This same point has been made periodically over the past fifty years. It has not been sufficiently realized that a true psychoanalytic understanding of resistance analysis could only begin with Freud's second theory of anxiety. Freud himself never fully embraced this theory, and clinical contributions since then have varied in their ability to use the techniques inherent in the second theory of anxiety. Recent contributions to the literature have not eliminated the espousal of theories of resistance based on earlier views of anxiety. Reasons cited for this include: the ambiguities in Freud's writing, the direction of the early ego theorists, and proclivities toward deeper interpretations. 相似文献
27.
Y M Agazarian 《International journal of group psychotherapy》1992,42(2):177-203
The author discusses how subgroups represent the basic focus of the systems-centered group therapist. Particular attention is given to boundary issues and to how therapy takes place by facilitating the process of discriminating, communicating, and integrating perception of differences in the apparently similar and similarities in the seemingly different. Communication occurs at the boundaries between systems at all levels of the hierarchy: the group, the subgroup, and its members. Clinical vignettes are provided to illustrate important process issues, such as work with difficult patients, levels of intervention, and group resistance and defense. 相似文献
28.
29.
Humans and monkeys show intermittent arm movements while tracking moving targets. This intermittency has been explained by postulating either a psychological refractory period after each movement and/or an error deadzone, an area surrounding the target within which movements are not initiated. We present a technique to detect and quantify the size of this deadzone, using a compensatory tracking paradigm that distinguishes it from a psychological refractory period. An artificial deadzone of variable size was added around a visual target displayed on a computer screen. While the subject was within this area, he received visual feedback that showed him to be directly on target. The presence of this artificial deadzone could affect tracking performance only if it exceeded the size of his intrinsic deadzone. Therefore, the size of artificial deadzone at which performance began to be affected revealed the size of the intrinsic deadzone. Measured at the subjects' eye, the deadzone was found to vary between 0.06 and 0.38 degrees, depending on the tracking task and viewing conditions; on the screen, this range was 1.3 mm to 3.3 mm. It increased with increasing speed of the target, with increasing viewing distance, and when the amplitude of the movement required was reduced. However, the deadzone size was not significantly correlated with the subjects' level of performance. We conclude that an intrinsic deadzone exists during compensatory tracking, and we suggest that its size is set by a cognitive process not simply related to the difficulty of the tracking task. 相似文献
30.
Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
A meta-analysis of research on Zajonc's (1968) mere exposure effect indicated that stimuli perceived without awareness produce substantially larger exposure effects than do stimuli that are consciously perceived (Bornstein, 1989a). However, this finding has not been tested directly in the laboratory. Two experiments were conducted comparing the magnitude of the exposure effect produced by 5-ms (i.e., subliminal) stimuli and stimuli presented for longer durations (i.e., 500 ms). In both experiments, 5-ms stimuli produced significantly larger mere exposure effects than did 500-ms stimuli. These results were obtained for polygon (Experiment 1), Welsh figure (Experiment 2), and photograph stimuli (Experiments 1 and 2). Implications of these findings for theoretical models of the mere exposure effect are discussed. 相似文献