首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   18746篇
  免费   721篇
  国内免费   5篇
  19472篇
  2019年   270篇
  2018年   341篇
  2017年   293篇
  2016年   328篇
  2015年   233篇
  2014年   322篇
  2013年   1549篇
  2012年   530篇
  2011年   579篇
  2010年   367篇
  2009年   369篇
  2008年   490篇
  2007年   530篇
  2006年   506篇
  2005年   408篇
  2004年   444篇
  2003年   385篇
  2002年   379篇
  2001年   561篇
  2000年   622篇
  1999年   415篇
  1998年   231篇
  1997年   186篇
  1996年   191篇
  1995年   179篇
  1992年   318篇
  1991年   338篇
  1990年   337篇
  1989年   317篇
  1988年   310篇
  1987年   265篇
  1986年   267篇
  1985年   283篇
  1984年   218篇
  1983年   229篇
  1982年   177篇
  1980年   173篇
  1979年   269篇
  1978年   228篇
  1976年   173篇
  1975年   233篇
  1974年   260篇
  1973年   291篇
  1972年   236篇
  1971年   229篇
  1970年   184篇
  1969年   212篇
  1968年   271篇
  1967年   263篇
  1966年   223篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
A scale assessing generalized false self‐perceptions (Perceptions of False Self, POFS) was developed and tested across three studies involving a total of 331 adolescents (11–16 years). In Study 1, interviews were conducted to develop items for the scale. In Study 2, psychometric techniques were used to derive a scale composed of 16 items. Study 2 also assessed the validity of the scale: depressive symptoms were found to increase POFS across 10 weeks, whereas false self‐perceptions had a reciprocal effect on anxiety. In Study 3, the convergent validity of the POFS scale was established. Overall, the evidence suggests that the POFS scale is a reliable and valid measure of generalized false self‐perceptions.  相似文献   
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
Although it is currently popular to model human associative learning using connectionist networks, the mechanism by which their output activations are converted to probabilities of response has received relatively little attention. Several possible models of this decision process are considered here, including a simple ratio rule, a simple difference rule, their exponential versions, and a winner-take-all network. Two categorization experiments that attempt to dissociate these models are reported. Analogues of the experiments were presented to a single-layer, feed-forward, delta-rule network. Only the exponential ratio rule and the winner-take-all architecture, acting on the networks' output activations that corresponded to responses available on test, were capable of fully predicting the mean response results. In addition, unlike the exponential ratio rule, the winner-take-all model has the potential to predict latencies. Further studies will be required to determine whether latencies produced under more stringent conditions conform to the model's predictions.  相似文献   
28.
29.
Native Hawaiian high school students, N = 1779, were surveyed for symptoms of psycho-pathology and suicide attempts in the previous 6 months. Seventy-seven (4.3%) of the students reported making a suicide attempt. There were no significant differences in prevalence rates for males and females. Depression, anxiety, aggression, substance abuse symptoms, and low family support, but not peer support, were significantly correlated with suicide attempts. On logistic regression, depression, substance abuse, and family support independently predicted attempts. The lack of gender difference may indicate a cultural characteristic of the Hawaiian population that differentiates it from mainstream American populations but likens it to the Native American population.  相似文献   
30.
The naming latency of a pronoun was measured when a single previously presented name in a discourse either agreed or did not agree with the pronoun in gender and person. An effect of agreement was found both under conditions in which subjects were likely to have engaged in strategic processing of the pronoun (Experiment 1) and under conditions in which this was unlikely (Experiment 3). The effect of gender agreement was also investigated when two noun phrases were present in the discourse. The results continued to show an immediate effect of gender agreement (naming latencies increased when a pronoun did not agree with one of two previously presented nouns) under experimental conditions likely to engender strategic processing (Experiment 2). This last effect was not significant under experimental conditions that were not likely to engender strategic processing (Experiment 3). The results are discussed in terms of models of the process of identifying the referent of a pronoun in a discourse.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号