首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   213篇
  免费   20篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   2篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   2篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   19篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   11篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   7篇
  2005年   5篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   6篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   2篇
  1937年   1篇
排序方式: 共有234条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
The traditional view holds that aggressive individuals have unfavorable views of self (i.e., low self-esteem), but recent studies indicate that the opposite might be true (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996). This debate may be informed by considering how individuals view others. Following relational schema theory (Baldwin, 1992), which suggests that the combination of self and other views is particularly important in predicting behavior in social situations, we examined self- and other-esteem, view of self in relation to views of others, and different forms of aggression among 125 young adults. Views of both self and others were associated with covert aggression (anger and hostility), whereas only views of others were correlated with overt aggression (physical and verbal). We also explored the discrepancy between views of self and other and found participants with more favorable views of self than others, and those who had equally unfavorable views of both self and others to be most aggressive. Findings suggest that views of both self and other and the relation among these views should be considered in future research on aggression.  相似文献   
192.
Knowledge of factors affecting eyewitness accuracy was examined in a sample of jurors, judges and law enforcement professionals. Participants completed a survey in which they were asked to agree or disagree with 30 statements about eyewitness issues, and their responses were compared to a sample of eyewitness experts who completed the same survey. Participant responses differed significantly from responses of eyewitness experts. Jurors disagreed with the experts on 87% of the issues, while judges and law enforcement disagreed with the experts on 60% of the issues. The findings show a large deficiency in knowledge of eyewitness memory amongst jurors, judges and law enforcement personnel, indicating that the legal system may benefit from expert assistance in the evaluation of eyewitness evidence. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
193.
本文介绍一个通过样例来学习解二元一次联立方程的计算机学习系统。系统根据人提供的各种样例,建立一系列产生式规则。这些规则组成了解方程组的知识,从而达到了学习和获得知识的目的。建立和应用产生式规则,也是人类学习和解决问题的一般形式。因此,关于计算机学习问题的研究,也将有助于理解人类学习的心理机制。  相似文献   
194.
A catalog of 119 studies, mostly post-1960 except for a few important and well-controlled earlier ones, is listed. The studies provide norms, scales, ratings, or lists of verbal material for use in experiments investigating aspects of cognitive psychology, memory, psycholinguistics, neuropsychology, etc. The catalog is preceded by an outline index.  相似文献   
195.
Outline drawings of human faces were presented for rating as to a number of dichotomous attributes, while the dimensions of the four constituent features were varied sysrematically. It was found that people act in a regular and predictable fashion in attributing characterisrics to certain of these features. When independently significant features appear in combination, there is a lawful increase in such characterizing responses. Subsidiary findings related to a halo effect between certain attributes, differential employment of some attributes, and a difference in overall strength of effect between certain features.  相似文献   
196.
Subjects made timed manual responses in judging whether laterally presented four-letter words were identical to targets. In Experiment 1, nontargets differed by a single letter from targets. A right-field superiority occurred only for targets (which were detected fastest of all) and for nontargets where a letter changed at Position 2 or Position 3. Changes at initial and final positions were detected faster than the two middle positions, and there were no significant field differences. In Experiment 2, ascenders and descenders were controlled and changes were made in nontargets at all four letter positions, at Positions 1 and 4, at Positions 2 and 3, or at 2 alone. Response times for nontargets varied inversely with the number of differing letters, regardless of position. Significant field differences again only appeared for changes in the two middle positions. Letters at the beginning and end of a word seem to be processed faster than and differently from those within, where field differences are strongest. Vowel-consonant differences probably do not account for these effects, which are more compatible with some form of parallel, rather than either serial or holistic, processing.  相似文献   
197.
Two experiments examined the effects of various feedback conditions on performance by 12 Ss on an electronic organ. Playing time scores showed complete absence of auditory feedback to have little or no detrimental effect; in fact, some Ss played fastest under this condition. Delayed auditory feedback (DAF), as expected, caused considerable performance decrement when compared with immediate auditory feedback (IAF). Extraneous prerecorded input as a distractor in the absence of auditory feedback ranked between mixed DAF-with-IAF, and pure DAF, which showed the greatest disruption. The possible role of auditory imagery in musical performance was discussed, together with four components which perhaps contribute to the standard DAF effect: distraction, error repetition, conflict with expectancies and auditory imagery, and presentation of two competing and conflicting signals (IAF and DAF).  相似文献   
198.
A device is described that accurately measures duration of phonation or articulation without being reset by intervening silent periods or pauses, which may be within the range of 10 to 1,000 msec. Thus, it is possible to time the overall duration of single sounds, syllables, single or multiple words, even though periods of silence are included. Input can be direct or recorded, the ancilliaries being a voice-operated switch and a chronoscope.  相似文献   
199.
The effect of punishment on free-operant choice behavior in humans   总被引:6,自引:6,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
During Phase I, three female human subjects pressed a button for monetary reinforcement in five variable-interval schedules specifying different frequencies of reinforcement. On alternate days, responding was also punished (by subtracting money) according to a variable-ratio 34 schedule. In the absence of punishment, response rates conformed to Herrnstein's equation for single variable-interval schedules. Punishment suppressed responding at all frequencies of reinforcement. This was reflected in a change in the values of both constants in Herrnstein's equation: the value of the theoretical maximum response-rate parameter was reduced, and the parameter describing the reinforcement frequency corresponding to the half-maximal response rate was elevated. During Phase II, the same five schedules (A) were in operation (without punishment), but in addition, a concurrent variable-interval schedule (B) of standard reinforcement frequency was introduced. On alternate days, responding in Component B was punished according to a variable-ratio 34 schedule. In the absence of punishment, absolute response rates conformed to equations proposed by Herrnstein to describe performance in concurrent schedules; the ratios of the response rates in the two components and the ratios of the times spent in the two components conformed to the Matching Law. When responding in Component B was punished, response rates in Component B were reduced and those in Component A were elevated, these changes being reflected in distortions of the matching relationship.  相似文献   
200.
Right-handed subjects were tachistoscopically presented three-letter words and nonword permutations in a single visual field for 20 msec. Although the subjects were unable to identify the stimuli at this exposure duration, their lexical decisions were better than chance, and were superior in the left visual field. When the exposure duration was increased, subjects demonstrated superior identification of the words in the right visual field. Results are discussed within an information processing framework.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号