全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1514篇 |
免费 | 97篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
1615篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 42篇 |
2018年 | 64篇 |
2017年 | 66篇 |
2016年 | 69篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 45篇 |
2013年 | 178篇 |
2012年 | 83篇 |
2011年 | 51篇 |
2010年 | 55篇 |
2009年 | 46篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 66篇 |
2006年 | 59篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 50篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1615条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Progressive muscle relaxation (PMR) is a broadly useful anxiety reduction technique that has been found to lower blood pressure (BP) in essential hypertension. The present investigation is the first to evaluate the feasibility and efficacy of wide-scale PMR instruction as a public health promotion strategy aimed at adolescents. Students (N = 1,400) in Grades 9 and 10 at two large Baltimore City public high schools underwent BP screening; those with BP persistently above the 85th percentile were randomly assigned to (a) 12 weeks daily PMR instruction provided in class for academic credit or (b) a control condition. The latter students had their BP measured at the same frequency as the PMR students and completed the PMR course during the following year. Approximately two thirds of eligible students entered and completed the study. Results demonstrated that treatment students (n = 51) enjoyed daily PMR, mastered the technique, and achieved reduced systolic BP at posttest relative to the untrained controls (n = 59). At follow-up 4 months later, group BP differences were not significant. Implications for use of PMR to promote cardiovascular health are discussed. 相似文献
42.
Chase and Simon's chunking theory of expert memory, which emphasizes the role of pattern recognition in problem solving,
has attracted much attention in cognitive psychology. Holding advanced a series of criticisms that, taken together, purported
to refute the theory. Two valid criticisms – that chunk size and LTM encoding were underestimated – are dealt with by a simple
extension of the theory. The remainder of Holding's criticisms either are not empirically founded or are based on a misunderstanding
of the chunking theory and its role in a comprehensive theory of skill. Holding's alternative SEEK theory, which emphasizes
the role of search, lacks key mechanisms that could be implemented by the type of pattern recognition proposed by Chase and
Simon.
Received: 3 September 1997 / Accepted: 28 January 1998 相似文献
43.
44.
Simon M. Huttegger 《Erkenntnis》2007,66(3):409-436
Recently there has been some interest in studying the explanation of meaning by using signaling games. I shall argue that
the meaning of signals in signaling games remains sufficiently unclear to motivate further investigation. In particular, the
possibility of distinguishing imperatives and indicatives at a fundamental level will be explored. Thereby I am trying to
preserve the generality of the signaling games framework while bringing it closer to human languages. A number of convergence
results for the evolutionary dynamics of our models will be proved. 相似文献
45.
McCullough LB McGuire AL Whitney SN 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2007,7(12):49-50; discussion W3-4
46.
This paper examines whether workplace characteristics, particularly resource availability and transformational leadership, moderate the association between openness to experience and organizational commitment. In Study 1, 205 employees completed a questionnaire that assessed their openness, organizational commitment, and perceived availability of resources at their organization. Openness reduced the extent to which individuals felt obliged to remain loyal to the organization, although this association diminished as resources became more accessible. In Study 2, 312 employees completed a similar questionnaire that also gauged the perceived leadership style of their supervisors. Transformational leadership amplified the positive relationship between openness and organizational commitment. These findings align with the proposition that individuals are more committed in work environments that facilitate the expression of their traits. 相似文献
47.
Visual cortical areas are involved in a variety of somatosensory tasks in the sighted, including tactile perception of two-dimensional patterns and motion, and haptic perception of three-dimensional objects. It is still unresolved whether visual imagery or modality-independent representations can better explain such cross-modal recruitment. However, these explanations are not necessarily in conflict with each other and might both be true, if imagery processes can access modality-independent representations. Greater visual cortical engagement in blind compared to sighted people is commonplace during language tasks, and also seems to occur during processing of tactile spatial information. Such engagement is even greater in the congenitally blind compared to the late blind, indicative of enhanced cross-modal plasticity during early development. At the other extreme, short-term visual deprivation of the normally sighted also leads to cross-modal plasticity. Altogether, the boundaries between sensory modalities appear to be flexible rather than immutable. 相似文献
48.
Hadjichristidis C Handley SJ Sloman SA Evans JS Over DE Stevenson RJ 《Memory & cognition》2007,35(8):2052-2059
The ability to entertain possibilities and draw inferences about them is essential to human intelligence. We examine the hypothesis that conditional if-then statements trigger a mental simulation process in which people suppose the antecedent (if statement) to be true and evaluate the consequent (then statement) in that context. On the assumption that supposing an event to be true increases belief that the event has occurred or will occur, this hypothesis is consistent with the claim that evaluating a conditional will heighten belief in its antecedent more than in its consequent. Two experiments, employing conditionals of the form If animal A has property X, then animal B will have property X, in which X was a property that people could not readily relate to the animals, supported this claim. The effect was stronger following the evaluation of conditionals with dissimilar animal categories. 相似文献
49.
50.
Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Tony Charman Simon Baron-Cohen John Swettenham Gillian Baird Antony Cox Auriol Drew 《Cognitive development》2000,15(4):1111
Various theoretical accounts propose that an important developmental relation exists between joint attention, play, and imitation abilities, and later theory of mind ability. However, very little direct empirical evidence supports these claims for putative “precursor” theory of mind status. A small sample (N=13) of infants, for whom measures of play, joint attention, and imitation had been collected at 20 months of age, was followed-up longitudinally at 44 months and a battery of theory of mind measures was conducted. Language and IQ were measured at both timepoints. Imitation ability at 20 months was longitudinally associated with expressive, but not receptive, language ability at 44 months. In contrast, only the joint attention behaviours of gaze switches between an adult and an active toy and looking to an adult during an ambiguous goal detection task at 20 months were longitudinally associated with theory of mind ability at 44 months. It is argued that joint attention, play, and imitation, and language and theory of mind, might form part of a shared social–communicative representational system in infancy that becomes increasingly specialised and differentiated as development progresses. 相似文献