首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1635篇
  免费   100篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2023年   18篇
  2022年   23篇
  2021年   21篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   42篇
  2018年   64篇
  2017年   66篇
  2016年   75篇
  2015年   41篇
  2014年   52篇
  2013年   184篇
  2012年   92篇
  2011年   56篇
  2010年   58篇
  2009年   47篇
  2008年   57篇
  2007年   74篇
  2006年   61篇
  2005年   46篇
  2004年   58篇
  2003年   58篇
  2002年   48篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   31篇
  1999年   27篇
  1998年   25篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   12篇
  1992年   18篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   12篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   11篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   9篇
  1979年   9篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   11篇
  1973年   10篇
  1971年   9篇
  1970年   11篇
  1968年   8篇
  1967年   9篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有1739条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
961.
Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We distinguish diagrammatic from sentential paper-and-pencil representations of information by developing alternative models of information-processing systems that are informationally equivalent and that can be characterized as sentential or diagrammatic. Sentential representations are sequential, like the propositions in a text. Diagrammatic representations are indexed by location in a plane. Diagrammatic representations also typically display information that is only implicit in sentential representations and that therefore has to be computed, sometimes at great cost, to make it explicit for use. We then contrast the computational efficiency of these representations for solving several illustrative problems in mathematics and physics. When two representations are informationally equivalent, their computational efficiency depends on the information-processing operators that act on them. Two sets of operators may differ in their capabilities for recognizing patterns, in the inferences they can carry out directly, and in their control strategies (in particular, the control of search). Diagrammatic and sentential representations support operators that differ in all of these respects. Operators working on one representation may recognize features readily or make inferences directly that are difficult to realize in the other representation. Most important, however, are differences in the efficiency of search for information and in the explicitness of information. In the representations we call diagrammatic, information is organized by location, and often much of the information needed to make an inference is present and explicit at a single location. In addition, cues to the next logical step in the problem may be present at an adjacent location. Therefore problem solving can proceed through a smooth traversal of the diagram, and may require very little search or computation of elements that had been implicit.  相似文献   
962.
963.
964.
In spite of policies advocating the involvement of families in the care of mental health service users in the UK, there are few examples of initiatives to develop staff confidence and skills in partnership working. This article describes a whole team training initiative and family liaison service to promote family inclusive working on in‐patient wards for older people in Somerset, UK. A three‐day staff‐training programme is described and training outcomes are reported. Staff report a substantial increase in confidence and family meetings held. A pre‐and post‐ training case note audit shows increased consideration of the needs of families. To further increase face to face meetings with families a family liaison service has been established, whereby a staff member with systemic family therapy training joins ward staff to hold family meetings as part of the assessment/admission process. Evaluation of this service has shown it to be effective with positive feedback from families and staff.  相似文献   
965.
966.
967.
968.
This paper presents three studies concerned with the evaluation of moves in solutions to Tower of Hanoi problems and the effect that such evaluation processes have on solution success. The existing literature on problem solving suggests that verbalizing whilst solving a problemcan havea positive effect upon performance. However,such verbalization has to be directed toward an explicit evaluation of particular moves. What remains unclear is whether evaluation without verbalization has the same effects or whether some characteristic of the process of verbalization gives rise to improved performance on such tasks. For example, the act of verbalizing per se may simply mean that more processing time is directed toward the problem-solving process. The studies reported in this paper suggest that the process of evaluation may be independent of verbalization processes and that non-verbal evaluation of moves (indicated by a key press) produces the same effects as a verbal evaluation of such moves. Moreover, the process of evaluating moves appears to producea form of behaviour that is prone to disruption via the administration of secondary tasks, whereas non-evaluated solutions are not. This may suggest that problem solvers who engage in evaluation processes develop an explicit representation of the strategies used to solve the problem.  相似文献   
969.
970.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号