首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1691篇
  免费   99篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2023年   18篇
  2022年   23篇
  2021年   21篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   44篇
  2018年   66篇
  2017年   68篇
  2016年   77篇
  2015年   40篇
  2014年   49篇
  2013年   212篇
  2012年   91篇
  2011年   53篇
  2010年   56篇
  2009年   50篇
  2008年   54篇
  2007年   71篇
  2006年   63篇
  2005年   46篇
  2004年   57篇
  2003年   57篇
  2002年   55篇
  2001年   34篇
  2000年   41篇
  1999年   33篇
  1998年   30篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   10篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   12篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   13篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   12篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   10篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   9篇
  1976年   9篇
  1975年   9篇
  1973年   10篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   10篇
  1967年   9篇
排序方式: 共有1794条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
991.
The following manifesto has been prepared by five prominent workers in the field of MCDA. We are currently preparing a special issue on the issues that they raise (see Call for Papers on p. 128). We invite you to contribute to the debate in that issue, either in the form of a paper or a letter to the Editor.  相似文献   
992.
Memory and planning processes in solutions to well-structured problems   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Although many studies in the problem-solving literature have considered the factors that might determine the strategies that are employed to solve well-structured problems, these have typically focused upon variants of means-end analysis. In general, such models imply that strategies unfold in a temporally forward direction, that problem solvers typically restrict forward-planning activities to just one or two moves ahead of the current problem state, and that one important heuristic is the avoidance of previous moves. Although studies have demonstrated the importance of such anti-looping heuristics, few have systematically explored the possibility that problem solvers may also plan retrospectively in order to try and assess whether a move might take them back to a state that they have previously visited. Those models of problem solving that promote the role of an anti-looping heuristic have assumed that the ability to use such a heuristic is based upon memory for previous states, but other interpretations are possible. In this paper several studies are reported that attempt systematically to explore participants' attempts to recognize previously visited problem solving states. The findings suggest that there is a systematic relationship between the success of this process, the time taken to make this judgement, and distance from the current state. It is also demonstrated that estimations about where future positions are likely to occur are symmetrical to estimations about past positions. It is suggested that this provides evidence that problem solvers engage in retrospective planning processes in order to try and avoid previous moves, and that this strategy may not be based straightforwardly upon their ability to remember previous problem states.  相似文献   
993.
994.
Hanne Andersen 《Erkenntnis》2000,52(3):313-337
It is a commonly raised argument against thefamily resemblance account of concepts that, on thisaccount, there is no limit to a concept's extension.An account of family resemblance which attempts toprovide a solution to this problem by including bothsimilarity among instances and dissimilarity tonon-instances has been developed by the philosopher ofscience Thomas Kuhn. Similar solutions have beenhinted at in the literature on family resemblanceconcepts, but the solution has never received adetailed investigation. I shall provide areconstruction of Kuhn's theory and argue that hissolution necessitates a developmental perspective withbuilds on both the transmission of taxonomies betweengenerations and a progressive development throughhistory.  相似文献   
995.
Reply to Lassiter   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
999.
1000.
In spite of policies advocating the involvement of families in the care of mental health service users in the UK, there are few examples of initiatives to develop staff confidence and skills in partnership working. This article describes a whole team training initiative and family liaison service to promote family inclusive working on in‐patient wards for older people in Somerset, UK. A three‐day staff‐training programme is described and training outcomes are reported. Staff report a substantial increase in confidence and family meetings held. A pre‐and post‐ training case note audit shows increased consideration of the needs of families. To further increase face to face meetings with families a family liaison service has been established, whereby a staff member with systemic family therapy training joins ward staff to hold family meetings as part of the assessment/admission process. Evaluation of this service has shown it to be effective with positive feedback from families and staff.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号