全文获取类型
收费全文 | 37393篇 |
免费 | 718篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
38119篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 255篇 |
2018年 | 3706篇 |
2017年 | 3030篇 |
2016年 | 2497篇 |
2015年 | 393篇 |
2014年 | 371篇 |
2013年 | 1512篇 |
2012年 | 1058篇 |
2011年 | 2866篇 |
2010年 | 2789篇 |
2009年 | 1791篇 |
2008年 | 2116篇 |
2007年 | 2614篇 |
2006年 | 497篇 |
2005年 | 585篇 |
2004年 | 574篇 |
2003年 | 488篇 |
2002年 | 425篇 |
2001年 | 507篇 |
2000年 | 588篇 |
1999年 | 379篇 |
1998年 | 246篇 |
1997年 | 188篇 |
1996年 | 186篇 |
1995年 | 177篇 |
1992年 | 291篇 |
1991年 | 293篇 |
1990年 | 312篇 |
1989年 | 286篇 |
1988年 | 278篇 |
1987年 | 245篇 |
1986年 | 250篇 |
1985年 | 269篇 |
1984年 | 201篇 |
1983年 | 215篇 |
1982年 | 167篇 |
1981年 | 159篇 |
1980年 | 163篇 |
1979年 | 256篇 |
1978年 | 223篇 |
1975年 | 224篇 |
1974年 | 247篇 |
1973年 | 278篇 |
1972年 | 232篇 |
1971年 | 218篇 |
1970年 | 172篇 |
1969年 | 201篇 |
1968年 | 255篇 |
1967年 | 248篇 |
1966年 | 213篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
52.
Traits and states are concepts that people use to both describe and understand themselves and others. We show that people view these concepts as prototype-based categories that have a graded internal structure and fuzzy boundaries and identify a set of attributes that define the prototypical cores of two categories: traits and states. Prototypical traits are stable, long-lasting, and internally caused. Prototypical states are temporary, brief, and caused by external circumstances. These prototypes are not defined by averages, as the family-resemblance principle would suggest, but by ideal (or extreme) attribute values. Like other ideal-based categories, traits and states serve particular goals. Trait concepts permit people to predict the present from the past; state concepts identify those behaviors that can be controlled by manipulating the situation. These two complementary schemas are part of the extensive theory of psychological causality that is implicit in language. Abstract social concepts differ from object categories in their category standards, the nature of their attributes, and the interrelations among those attributes. 相似文献
53.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
54.
F. Charles Mace Michael L. Hock Joseph S. Lalli Barbara J. West Phillip Belfiore Elizabeth Pinter D. Kirby Brown 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1988,21(2):123-141
Behavioral momentum refers to the tendency for behavior to persist following a change in environmental conditions. The greater the rate of reinforcement, the greater the behavioral momentum. The intervention for noncompliance consisted of issuing a sequence of commands with which the subject was very likely to comply (i.e., high-probability commands) immediately prior to issuing a low-probability command. In each of five experiments, the high-probability command sequence resulted in a “momentum” of compliant responding that persisted when a low-probability request was issued. Results showed the antecedent high-probability command sequence increased compliance and decreased compliance latency and task duration. “Momentum-like” effects were shown to be distinct from experimenter attention and to depend on the contiguity between the high-probability command sequence and the low-probability command. 相似文献
55.
56.
We investigated the possible influence of common "demand characteristics" on the results on behavioral language laterality tasks. The demand characteristics were (1) whether Ss were or were not recruited for study according to their handedness, and (2) whether a detailed familial sinistrality inquiry was conducted before or after the tasks. Tasks were the Dichotic Consonant Vowel Task (DCVT) and the Bilateral Object Naming Latency Task (BONLT). Results showed no effect of demand characteristics on the BONLT. On the DCVT, however, prior inquiry regarding familial sinistrality was associated with significantly reduced right ear advantages (REAs). Interactions showed that the greatest reduction in REAs occurred in Ss with left-handedness in their families, who were asked about familial sinistrality prior to the tasks, who took the DCVT after the BONLT, and who were also recruited by handedness. Results, though generally reassuring regarding the robustness of the tasks against these sources of bias, raise some cautions for the DCVT. 相似文献
57.
58.
Body-frame size is an important factor in determining an optimal body weight for a given height. Previous studies have indicated that many individuals incorrectly estimate their body-frame size, and, as a result, incorrectly assess their ideal weight. The present study investigated the accuracy of estimation of body-frame size as a function of sex and actual frame size. The subjects were 66 men and 52 women participating in a community adult fitness program. Data indicated that medium-framed individuals were the most accurate in their estimations of body-frame size. Also, women were twice as likely to be accurate as were men. These results are interpreted to mean that most people assume they are medium-framed and that there is a sex difference in the way body-frame size is estimated. 相似文献
59.
Cutoff scores are used in a variety of settings to help managers make personnel decisions. Although cutoff scores are used widely, information about their derivation and appropriate use is scattered in the legal, psychometric, and professional literatures. The purpose of this paper is to analyze critically and to integrate these diverse literatures, to summarize what is known and what is unknown about the use and misuse of cutoff scores. Alternative methods are examined in relation to legal precedents set by the courts, to psychometric principles, and to the various sets of professional guidelines. Directions for future research are suggested, and we conclude with a set of recommendations regarding acceptable professional practice in this area. 相似文献
60.
S Martin K Housley H Mccoy P Greenhouse F Stigger M A Kenney S Shoffner V Fu M Korslund F G Ercanli-Huffman 《Perceptual and motor skills》1988,67(3):879-884
The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale was administered to 550 14- and 16-yr.-old (+/- 6 mo.) girls. Self-esteem scores were categorized by weight and weight by height. Scores on the Quetelet Index for obesity were correlated with self-esteem scores. Mean self-esteem of the low- and middle-weight by height group was significantly higher than the mean of the high-weight by height group. In analyzing weight alone, the self-esteem of the middle-weight group was significantly higher than the self-esteem of the high-weight group. The correlation of the obesity index and self-esteem indicated that as weight increased self-esteem decreased. 相似文献