全文获取类型
收费全文 | 583篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
603篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 39篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 63篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
1950年 | 2篇 |
1933年 | 1篇 |
1931年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有603条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
The current study investigated the influence of rewards on very young children's helping behavior. After 20-month-old infants received a material reward during a treatment phase, they subsequently were less likely to engage in further helping during a test phase as compared with infants who had previously received social praise or no reward at all. This so-called overjustification effect suggests that even the earliest helping behaviors of young children are intrinsically motivated and that socialization practices involving extrinsic rewards can undermine this tendency. 相似文献
242.
Isabel L. Silva José Pais-Ribeiro Helena Cardoso 《Applied research in quality of life》2008,3(2):127-136
The purpose of this study was to determine the differences in general health status and quality of life between patients who
present “non-normative eating behaviours” (namely, binge eating disorder, night eating syndrome, sweet /fat food cravings
and continuous nibbling) and patients without these eating behaviour characteristics. A cohort of 168 women with obesity,
all candidates to bariatric surgery, aged between 15 and 65 (M = 39.04; SD = 10.86), with a body mass index ranging between
30.37 and 80.65 (M = 48.79; SD = 8.37), was studied. Participants answered to a clinical interview, to SF-36 and to ORWELL-97,
after their informed consent. Results suggest that women with obesity reporting binge eating disorder, night eating syndrome
and continuous nibbling show poorer general health status and worst quality of life than those who do not present these “non-normative
eating behaviours”. Moreover, the general health status and the quality of life of patients with sweet/fat food cravings do
not differ from those who do not report this eating behaviour. Nevertheless, patients reporting the eating behaviours studied
do not differ from the others regarding their body mass index. 相似文献
243.
Paul Silva Jr. 《Pacific Philosophical Quarterly》2017,98(1):25-48
According to many, to have epistemic justification to believe P is just for it to be epistemically permissible to believe P. Others think it is for believing P to be epistemically good. Yet others think it has to do with being epistemically blameless in believing P. All such views of justification encounter problems. Here, a new view of justification is proposed according to which justification is a kind of composite normative status. The result is a view of justification that offers hope of solving some longstanding epistemological problems. 相似文献
244.
Bill Henry Mike Feehan Rob McGee Warren Stanton Terrie E. Moffitt Phil Silva 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1993,21(5):469-480
The current study assessed the relative importance of conduct problems and depressive symptoms, measured at two ages (11 and 15), for predicting substance use at age 15 in an unselected birth cohort of New Zealand adolescents. Among males, when the relative predictive utility of both conduct problems and depressive symptoms was assessed, only pre-adolescent depressive symptoms were found to predict multiple drug use 4 years later. No predictive relation was found between early symptomatology and later substance use among females. The strongest association between predictors and substance use emerged between age 15 multiple drug use and concurrent conduct problems for both males and females. Finally, both conduct problems and depressive symptoms at age 15 were also found to be associated with concurrent self-medication among females.This work was supported by USPHS grants MH-43746 and MH-45070 from the Antisocial and Violent Behavior Branch of the National Institute of Mental Health to Terrie Moffitt. Bill Henry was funded by a Fulbright Travel Award. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit is supported by the Medical Research Council and Health Research Council of New Zealand. Appreciation is expressed to the interviewers who collected the data, and to the young New Zealanders who were members of the sample. 相似文献
245.
Although neighborhood stressors have a negative impact on youth, and social support can play a protective role, it is unclear
what types and sources of social support may contribute to positive outcomes among at-risk youth. We examined the influences
of neighborhood disadvantage and social support on global self-worth among low-income, urban African American youth, both
concurrently and longitudinally. We examined social support from both a structural and functional perspective, and tested
the main-effects and the stress-buffering models of social support. Participants included 82–130 youth, in 6th–8th grade,
who completed self-report measures. Network support results suggest participants received emotional, tangible, and informational
support most often from mothers and other female relatives, with friends, fathers, and teachers also playing important roles.
Model testing accounted for neighborhood stressors and support from various sources, revealing support from close friends
was associated with concurrent self-worth; whereas, parent support predicted self-worth longitudinally, above and beyond initial
levels of self-worth. The findings provide evidence for the main-effects model of social support and not the stress-buffering
model. Our findings illustrate the importance of extended family networks and the types of support that youth rely upon in
African American impoverished communities, as well as how support contributes to global self-worth. Implications and suggestions
for future research and intervention are discussed. 相似文献
246.
247.
Silva JR Pizzagalli DA Larson CL Jackson DC Davidson RJ 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2002,111(4):676-681
It is well known that the eating patterns that restrain chronic dieters (restrained eaters) can be disinhibited by anxiety, which in turn has been associated with relative right frontal brain activity in independent electroencephalographic (EEG) studies. Combining these two lines of evidence, the authors tested the hypothesis that chronic restrained eating is associated with relative right frontal asymmetry. Resting anterior brain asymmetry and self-reported measures of anxiety and depression were collected in 23 restrained and 32 unrestrained eaters. As hypothesized, groups differed in tonic frontal activity, with restrained eaters showing more relative right frontal activity. Furthermore, relative right frontal activity was associated with greater self-reported restraint. Right-sided prefrontal asymmetry may thus represent a diathesis associated with increased vulnerability toward restrained eating. 相似文献
248.
Attachment-related strategies are thought to be critical for regulation and processing of emotional information. This study examined biases in selective attention to emotional stimuli as a function of insecure attachment. Participants searched for a single target image preceded by to-be-ignored distracters depicting emotional images varying in valence and arousal. Results revealed that, in general, negative distracters affected accuracy levels, and that the anxious attached participants showed a clear interference of the emotional distracters. In contrast, the avoidant group evinced a higher control on such interference. In addition, arousal ratings to distracter images indicated superior emotional activation only for anxious attached participants. Consistent with the evolutionary-based attachment theory threat-related stimuli prompted priority attentional responses. Present findings are in line with evidence showing the deployment of distinct strategies in insecurely attached individuals for the regulation of attention to emotional information. 相似文献
249.
Jairo José da Silva 《Axiomathes》2012,22(1):5-30
Husserl left many unpublished drafts explaining (or trying to) his views on spatial representation and geometry, such as,
particularly, those collected in the second part of Studien zur Arithmetik und Geometrie (Hua XXI), but no completely articulate work on the subject. In this paper, I put forward an interpretation of what those
views might have been. Husserl, I claim, distinguished among different conceptions of space, the space of perception (constituted
from sensorial data by intentionally motivated psychic functions), that of physical geometry (or idealized perceptual space),
the space of the mathematical science of physical nature (in which science, not only raw perception has a word) and the abstract
spaces of mathematics (free creations of the mathematical mind), each of them with its peculiar geometrical structure. Perceptual
space is proto-Euclidean and the space of physical geometry Euclidean, but mathematical physics, Husserl allowed, may find
it convenient to represent physical space with a non-Euclidean structure. Mathematical spaces, on their turn, can be endowed,
he thinks, with any geometry mathematicians may find interesting. Many other related questions are addressed here, in particular
those concerning the a priori or a posteriori character of the many geometric features of perceptual space (bearing in mind
that there are at least two different notions of a priori in Husserl, which we may call the conceptual and the transcendental
a priori). I conclude with an overview of Weyl’s ideas on the matter, since his philosophical conceptions are often traceable
back to his former master, Husserl. 相似文献
250.
Context memories initially require the hippocampus, but over time become independent of this structure. This shift reflects a consolidation process whereby memories are gradually stored in distributed regions of the cortex. The function of this process is thought to be the extraction of statistical regularities and general knowledge from specific experiences. The current study examined this idea in mice by measuring the specificity of context memories during consolidation. In the first experiment, separate groups of animals were trained with a single shock and tested in the training context or a novel environment 1, 14, 28, or 36 d later. We found a systematic increase in generalization over this period. Initially, mice froze more in the training context, but fear of the novel environment grew over time until animals eventually froze an equivalent amount in both contexts. The second experiment demonstrated that the increase in generalization was due to a loss of detailed information about the context and not fear incubation. In this experiment, mice were exposed to the context and then trained with an immediate shock 1 or 36 d later. Animals trained 1 d after exposure acquired robust context fear that did not generalize across environments. In contrast, mice trained 36 d after exposure froze an equivalent amount in the training context and the novel environment. The same profile was observed in H-ras mutants that exhibit enhanced hippocampal plasticity and learning. These results suggest that context memories are specific early after training when they require the hippocampus, and become more general as they are permanently stored in the cortex. 相似文献