首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6063篇
  免费   1522篇
  国内免费   10篇
  2023年   44篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   111篇
  2020年   161篇
  2019年   450篇
  2018年   372篇
  2017年   479篇
  2016年   479篇
  2015年   392篇
  2014年   375篇
  2013年   821篇
  2012年   401篇
  2011年   349篇
  2010年   339篇
  2009年   298篇
  2008年   297篇
  2007年   241篇
  2006年   212篇
  2005年   207篇
  2004年   169篇
  2003年   176篇
  2002年   182篇
  2001年   142篇
  2000年   104篇
  1999年   70篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   32篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   34篇
  1994年   41篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   37篇
  1991年   27篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   28篇
  1987年   20篇
  1986年   27篇
  1985年   27篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   19篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   29篇
  1977年   18篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   18篇
  1973年   15篇
排序方式: 共有7595条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
12.
13.
14.
15.
Summary Network models of semantic memory assume implicitly or explicitly that the degree of activation of a node is a monotonic function of the total amount of excitation reaching that node from all sources. For example, the activation level of the node representing apple should be greater if it is receiving excitation due to the activation of the nodes for fruit and pear than if only one of these neighboring nodes is activated. This notion was tested by presenting semantic primes 80 ms or 320 ms before a letter string (e.g., apple or ipple) requiring a lexical decision. The prime stimuli consisted of a pair of simultaneous items that were identical in the single prime condition (e.g., fruit/fruit or pear/pear) or different in the double prime condition (e.g., fruit/pear or pear/fruit), and were either related (target = apple) or unrelated (target = copper) to the target, or neutral. As predicted by the summation of semantic activation assumption of network models there was a larger priming effect (in the 320 ms SOA condition) following the simultaneous activation of two related nodes than following the activation of only one node.This research was supported by grants to the first author from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.  相似文献   
16.
17.
18.
With approximately one out of six married couples experiencing infertility, having a basic knowledge of infertility is important to marriage and family therapists. This paper presents important themes that infertile couples often struggle with and stresses the importance of having a biological, psychological, and social understanding of infertility. In addition, the paper suggests possible interventions that family therapists can use in helping infertile couples.  相似文献   
19.
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号