全文获取类型
收费全文 | 290篇 |
免费 | 52篇 |
国内免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
368篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 29篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有368条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
291.
Roy R. Spina Li‐Jun Ji Michael Ross Ye Li Zhiyong Zhang 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》2010,13(3):153-162
Four studies were conducted to investigate cultural differences in predicting and understanding regression toward the mean. We demonstrated, with tasks in such domains as athletic competition, health and weather, that Chinese are more likely than Canadians to make predictions that are consistent with regression toward the mean. In addition, Chinese are more likely than Canadians to choose a regression‐consistent explanation to account for regression toward the mean. The findings are consistent with cultural differences in lay theories about how people, objects and events develop over time. 相似文献
292.
In this study, the authors evaluated the Chinese version of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI). The ZTPI was tested among a sample of 303 university students. A subsample of 51 participants was then asked to complete the ZTPI again along with another set of questionnaires. The five‐factor model of a 20‐item short version of the ZTPI showed good model fit, internal consistency, and test‐retest reliability. The 20‐item Chinese version of the ZTPI also provided good validity, showing correlations with other variables in expected directions. Past‐Positive was positively correlated with reappraisal and negatively correlated with suppression emotion regulation strategies, and Present‐Hedonistic was positively correlated with reappraisal emotion regulation strategies. These findings indicate that the ZTPI is a reliable and valid instrument for measuring time perspective in the Chinese setting. 相似文献
293.
Purchase and consumption behaviors in daily life often are repetitive and performed in customary places, leading consumers to develop habits. When habits have formed, environmental cues can activate the practiced responses in the absence of conscious decision making. This research tested these ideas using a longitudinal design. We predicted that regardless of their explicit intentions, consumers would repeat habits to purchase fast food, watch TV news, and take the bus. The results yielded the anticipated pattern in which participants repeated habitual behaviors even if they reported intentions to do otherwise. Intentions only guided behavior in the absence of strong habits. This study ruled out a number of artifactual accounts for these findings including that they arise from the level of abstraction at which intentions are identified, the certainty with which participants held intentions, a restriction of range in the measures, and the strategy participants used to estimate frequency of past performance. 相似文献
294.
社会价值取向与反馈对动态社会两难决策的影响:以水资源两难决策为例 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
以水资源两难情景为实验任务,探讨了社会价值取向与反馈在社会两难决策中的作用.结果表明: (1)社会价值取向在决策者的两难决策行为中起着非常重要的作用.合作型个体比非合作型个体显著地表现出更多的合作行为.个体的社会价值取向可以作为预测两难决策行为的一个重要因素: (2)不同的反馈类型对具有不同社会价值取向的决策者的决策行为影响不同.非合作型个体受反馈信息影响比较明显,而合作型个体则不易受他人的决策信息影响. 相似文献
295.
Jill A. Jacobson Li-Jun Ji Peter H. Ditto Zhiyong Zhang Dara H. Sorkin Sarah K. Warren 《Psychology & health》2013,28(10):1194-1210
Objective: The current studies examined if cultural and self-construal differences in self-enhancement extended to defensive responses to health threats. Design: Responses to fictitious medical diagnoses were compared between Asian-Americans and European-North Americans in Experiment 1 and between Canadians primed with an interdependent versus an independent self-construal in Experiment 3. In Experiment 2, the responses of Chinese and Canadians who were either heavy or light soft drink consumers were assessed after reading an article linking soft drink consumption to insulin resistance. Main outcome measure: The primary-dependent measure reflected participants’ defensiveness about threatening versus nonthreatening health information. Results: In Experiment 1, all participants responded more defensively to an unfavourable than a favourable diagnosis; however, Asian-Americans responded less defensively than did European-North Americans. In Experiment 2, all high soft drink consumers were less convinced by the threatening information than were low soft drink consumers; however, among high consumers, Chinese changed their self-reported consumption levels less than did European-Canadians. In Experiment 3, interdependence-primed participants responded less defensively to an unfavourable diagnosis than did independence-primed participants. Conclusion: Defensive reactions to threatening health information were found consistently; however, self-enhancement was more pronounced in individuals with Western cultural backgrounds or independent self-construals. 相似文献
296.
Precession electron diffraction (PED) has been carried out on a fully ordered Fe–40at.%Al solid solution in order to explore its suitability for determining the state of order. The integrated intensity ratio of a superlattice reflection with respect to a fundamental reflection was measured as a function of the sample thickness and the results are well fitted by dynamical simulations. It is proposed that, in addition to conventional X-ray diffraction, PED may provide access to the state of order at microscopic and nanoscopic scales. 相似文献
297.
Chang-Ho C. Ji Yodi Ibrahim 《The International journal for the psychology of religion》2013,23(3):189-208
A measure of Islamic doctrinal orthodoxy was developed, and its relation to personal religious activities was examined. This study also applied the concept of extrinsic, intrinsic, and quest religiosity to Islamic faith as it investigated the structural validity and internal consistency of Islamic versions of the religious orientation scales. A sample of 381 students was taken from three universities in Indonesia. Notably, the scales were found to be psychometrically adequate and their doctrinal orthodoxy and intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity scores significantly related to personal practice of religious activities. A subsequent analysis also revealed that, for Muslims, doctrinal beliefs are largely independent of extrinsic, intrinsic, and quest religiosity and that the three forms of religiosity are three autonomous but continuous dimensions of personal religiosity. 相似文献
298.
Michael K. Scullin Brian A. Gordon Jill Talley Shelton Ji hae Lee Denise Head Mark A. McDaniel 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2013,13(2):405-416
Hypertension affects many older adults and is associated with impaired neural and cognitive functioning. We investigated whether a history of hypertension was associated with impairments to prospective memory, which refers to the ability to remember to perform delayed intentions, such as remembering to take medication. Thirty-two cognitively normal older adult participants with or without a history of hypertension (self-reported) performed two laboratory prospective memory tasks, one that relied more strongly on executive control (nonfocal prospective memory) and one that relied more strongly on spontaneous memory retrieval processes (focal prospective memory). We observed hypertension-related impairments for nonfocal, but not focal, prospective memory. To complement our behavioral approach, we conducted a retrospective analysis of available structural magnetic resonance imaging data. Lower white matter volume estimates in the anterior prefrontal cortex were associated with lower nonfocal prospective memory and with a history of hypertension. A history of hypertension may be associated with worsened executive control and lower prefrontal white matter volume. The translational implication is that individuals who must remember to take antihypertensive medications and to monitor their blood pressure at home may be impaired in the executive control process that helps to support these prospective memory behaviors. 相似文献
299.
胡塞尔的触觉观是常常遭到忽视的一个主题。提到现象学直观,一般会将其与视觉联系起来。但在胡塞尔的诸多文献中,视觉与触觉经常成对出现,而且他认为触觉是元感觉场,强调触感知相对于视感知的优先性,进而将触摸行为视作实践活动的基础。不过,胡塞尔的这些表述大多是零散、不完善的。本文将通过触觉的双重立义特征来探讨其元感觉场地位,引出其作为元实践场的意涵;然后着眼于元实践区域中对象和主体两个维度的原初显现,将触觉优先性放入机械力和能力如何被给予和类比的问题中进行解释,以此对触觉在实践中的基础地位做一个新的系统性阐发。 相似文献
300.
In this article, we examine the hypothesis that problem comprehension and computational processes interact during the solving of an arithmetic word problem. Two experiments tested subjects on a series of addition and subtraction word problems, the content of which varied on the basis of problem type and on the magnitude of the numbers involved (problem size). Performance data are presented and analyzed in terms of solution reaction times and error patterns. Results confirmed the main effects of problem size and problem type as factors determining reaction times but failed to show any significant interaction between the two factors. These results suggest that the cognitive processes involved in understanding an arithmetic word problem and in performing the required computations are best explained by a serial processing model. The absence of an interaction between problem comprehension and computational processes questions the notion that automatized retrieval facilitates problem solving and assertions suggesting that increasing computational requirements can interfere with problem-solving performance. 相似文献