排序方式: 共有54条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
In this study, we examined the effects of cognitive task performance on the induction of vection. We hypothesized that, if vection requires attentional resources, performing cognitive tasks requiring attention should inhibit or weaken it. Experiment 1 tested the effects on vection of simultaneously performing a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task. The results revealed that the RSVP task affected the subjective strength of vection. Experiment 2 tested the effects of a multiple-object-tracking (MOT) task on vection. Simultaneous performance of the MOT task decreased the duration and subjective strength of vection. Taken together, these findings suggest that vection induction requires attentional resources. 相似文献
12.
In this paper, we will examine and untangle a conflict mainly between a developmental psychologist, Martin Hoffman and a social
psychologist, Daniel Batson. According to Hoffman, empathic distress, a vicarious feeling through empathy, is transformed
into an altruistic motivation. Batson and others on the other hand, criticize Hoffman, claiming that empathic altruism has
no relation with empathic distress. We will point out some problems with Batson’s position by referring to the results of
fMRI experiments that suggest empathic distress and empathic altruism share a common basis, and defend Hoffman’s argument.
This will also offer new insights into the evolution of empathy.
Hisashi Nakao obtained his BA and MA from Kyoto University. He is currently a graduate student at the Department of Philosophy and History of Science, the Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University. His main research interests are in philosophy of biology and psychology, especially philosophical issues in the evolution of human behaviors or psychology. Shoji Itakura obtained his BS from Yokohama National University, and MS and Ph.D. from Kyoto University, Primate Research Institute. He is currently an Associate Professor of the Department of Psychology, the Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University. His main research interests are in social cognition in infants and Developmental Cybernetics which he advocates as new research domain. 相似文献
Hisashi NakaoEmail: |
Hisashi Nakao obtained his BA and MA from Kyoto University. He is currently a graduate student at the Department of Philosophy and History of Science, the Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University. His main research interests are in philosophy of biology and psychology, especially philosophical issues in the evolution of human behaviors or psychology. Shoji Itakura obtained his BS from Yokohama National University, and MS and Ph.D. from Kyoto University, Primate Research Institute. He is currently an Associate Professor of the Department of Psychology, the Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University. His main research interests are in social cognition in infants and Developmental Cybernetics which he advocates as new research domain. 相似文献
13.
Duffy S Toriyama R Itakura S Kitayama S 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2009,102(3):351-359
Recent studies suggest that North American adults exhibit a focused strategy of attention that emphasizes focal information about objects, whereas Japanese adults exhibit a divided strategy of attention that emphasizes contextual information about objects. The current study investigated whether 4- and 5-, 6- to 8-, and 9- to 13-year-old North American and Japanese children exhibit these divergent attention strategies. Two experiments suggest that those older than 6 years of age exhibit measurable cultural differences in attention, whereas 4- to 6-year-olds do not. We suggest that sociocognitive development and socialization experiences that occur around 5 to 7 years of age may foster the development of cultural strategies of attention. 相似文献
14.
Shoji Itakura 《Animal cognition》2001,4(3-4):281-284
The goal of this study was to examine the effect of joint visual attention on infants' behavior during subsequent events.
Thirty-seven mother-infant (aged from 9 to 13 months) pairs were twice shown a pair of line drawing stimuli on a computer
screen. For the control group, the mother never paid attention to the stimuli. For experimental group 1, the mother pointed
to one stimulus in the first presentation but did not point to it in the second presentation. The infants gazed longer at
the stimulus pointed to by their mothers in the first presentation. In the second presentation, during which mothers did not
attend to the stimuli, infants gazed longer at the stimulus which had been pointed to by the mothers in the first presentation.
In experimental group 2, one of two stimuli blinked during the first presentation but not the second presentation. Infants
gazed for longer at the blinking stimulus in the first presentation, but there was no difference in looking time toward the
two stimuli in the second presentation. These results suggest that joint visual attention affects infants' looking behavior
during subsequent events, and that simple stimulus change does not.
Accepted after revision: 2 May 2001
Electronic Publication 相似文献
15.
Hand preferences of two Japanese macaques were observed in five differing tasks: simple reaching for food on a platform; choosing of boxes with pieces of apple; mirror-guided reaching (taking food from the box, key pressing, key tracking). Though these were visually guided tasks, the macaques showed different hand preferences.
相似文献16.
17.
Shoji Kakigi 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1971,6(4):191-204
The present study was designed to study generalization and differentiation and to check the relation between heart rate and blood pressure in cardiovascular conditioning. Five naive mongrel dogs were presented with 10 sec 1000 cps tone regularly reinforced with 0.5-sec shock to a left foreleg. Four other 10-sec tones (600, 800, 1200, 1400 cps), never reinforced, were administered in a randomized and counterbalanced fashion using a differential reinforcing technique. The response measures were heart rate and three indices of blood pressure,i.e., systolic, diastolic, and pulse pressures.“Initial response” of heart rate showed rapid tachycardia to all tones, with no generalization and differentiation among five tones.“Terminal response” clearly indicated generalization gradients that tachycardia to 800 and 1200 cps tones were higher than those to 600 and 1400 cps tones, respectively. Some differences were noted among systolic, diastolic, and pulse pressures, with pulse pressure showing a trend of generalization and differentiation in the“initial response.” No statistical difference, however, was obtained in blood pressure scores. Beat-by-beat analyses of correlations between heart rate and diastolic blood pressure showed that the correlations successively decreased during the“terminal response.” Overall correlations between heart rate and three indices of blood pressure were obtained, with the highest correlation between heart rate and pulse pressure. Considering the above results in relation to the classical conditioning theory, it was tentatively concluded that“initial response” includes non-conditional factors, while“terminal response” reflects mainly a true conditional response, particularly, at a long CS-US interval. This interpretation leads to the hypothesis that conditional response with a short CS-US interval may attain better conditioning scores in terms of the summation effect between a non-conditional factor and a true conditional factor. 相似文献
18.
Hiroshi Nonami Yukio Hirose Susumu Ohnuma Cees Midden Shoji Ohtomo 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》2015,18(3):216-224
In citizen participation, a few representatives of the total citizen population participate in discussions with authorities regarding public decisions and policies. The present study examines a dual process model in which the representatives’ voice and similarity of values facilitate public acceptance through procedural fairness and trust in representatives, respectively. The results of an experiment employing a scenario method, which included participants from Japan (n = 211) and the Netherlands (n = 200), indicate that the representatives’ voice increased procedural fairness and public acceptance when the similarity of representatives was high. The effects of representatives’ voice on public acceptance via procedural fairness was supported in both nations, while other effects of representatives’ similarity on acceptance via trust were indicated only in Japan. These results suggest that the indirect voice of citizens, as conveyed by representatives, plays an important role in increasing perceptions of procedural fairness and public acceptance among citizens. 相似文献
19.
Hayao Imashioya M.A. Andrew B. Dollins Ph.D. Shoji Kakigi Ph.D. 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1987,22(1):1-6
This study systematically investigates changes in CNV waveform shape and resolution time that result from the presentation of facilitatory, inhibitory, or no motor response (MR) information simultaneously with the warning (S1) or imperative (S2) stimulus of the S1-S2-MR CNV paradigm. Analyses indicate that the simultaneous presentation of S1 and information to produce or inhibit a MR attenuates initial CNV development. Further, when the S1 information is inhibitive, CNV development is retarded throughout. The contribution of an inhibitory psychological process during CNV development is proposed. The data also indicate that CNV resolution time is not dependent on the presence of a motor response. It is suggested that CNV resolution time is indicative of psychological completion or closure. 相似文献
20.
The present study examined whether young children's behaviors in the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task can be influenced by their observation of another person performing the task. Experiment 1 showed that after children watched an adult sorting cards according to one rule, although the children were instructed to sort the cards according to a new rule, most 3-year-olds made perseverative errors and used the observed, old rule to sort the cards instead of the new rule. However, only some 4-year-olds and few 5-year-olds made the same mistake. Experiments 2, 3 and 4 showed that the younger children took into consideration social pragmatic information displayed by the adult model when deciding to use the old rule or the new rule. When the model appeared to know that she sorted the cards incorrectly (Experiments 2 and 3), or was uncertain whether she sorted cards correctly (Experiment 4), most 3-year-olds no longer committed perseverative errors. When the adult model was confident about her sorting or oblivious to her sorting errors, most 3-year-olds made perseverative errors. These results taken together suggest that social observation can lead to disinhibitions. In other words, disinhibition can be transmitted socially from one person to another. 相似文献