全文获取类型
收费全文 | 154篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
158篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 16篇 |
2012年 | 9篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有158条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
R. K. Gupta 《International Journal of Philosophical Studies》2013,21(3):383-396
Abstract This article is concerned with examining Kant's derivation of the various formulae of his Categorical Imperative. It is in agreement with Paton in maintaining that Kant actually mentions five formulae. But it is not in agreement with him, and some others, in maintaining that they are ultimately reducible to three. Nor is it in agreement with those who maintain that they are ultimately reducible to just one. According to the present article, they are ultimately reducible to two: that about a moral law being a universal law; and that about a moral law involving treating human beings as ends in themselves. 相似文献
62.
Tina R. Norton Anita Gupta Mary Ann Parris Stephens Lynn M. Martire Aloen L. Townsend 《Sex roles》2005,52(5-6):325-335
Although research suggests that stress and rewards experienced in a social role are associated with changes in the centrality (or personal importance) of that role over time, little attention has been given to the mechanisms that account for this relationship. This study was conducted to examine change in role mastery as a mediator in the relationship between changes in role experiences (stress and rewards) and changes in centrality among 195 women who simultaneously occupied the roles of parent care provider, mother, wife, and employee. Regression analyses indicated that increases in parent care, mother, and employee stress eroded mastery in those roles. In addition, increases in rewards were associated with a bolstered sense of mastery in each of the four roles. Mastery was found to be a mediating mechanism in the relationship between stress/rewards and centrality in one of the four roles examined, the employee role. However, changes in womens perceptions of mastery were unrelated to the extent to which women changed the importance of their three family roles. These findings extend prior research by demonstrating that mastery is a mechanism through which stress and rewards are associated with centrality in the employee role. 相似文献
63.
Gupta P 《Memory (Hove, England)》2005,13(3-4):318-324
An increasing body of evidence suggests that nonword repetition is related to immediate serial memory (e.g., Baddeley, Gathercole, & Papagno, 1998; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993). One possible account of this relationship is that a nonword is processed like a list when it is first encountered. If this is the case, it should be possible to detect serial position effects in repetition of single nonwords. Three experiments tested this prediction. Experiment 1 examined whether there would be syllable serial position primacy and recency effects in repetition of polysyllabic nonwords, and obtained both primacy and recency effects. Experiments 2 and 3 showed that these effects were not due to the controlled duration of the nonwords or the requirements of concurrent articulation or the procedure by which nonwords were created. 相似文献
64.
This paper introduces the vision for the Phenomenological Film Collective ( pfcollective.com ), a research and filmmaking group which utilizes phenomenological research in the service of social advocacy filmmaking. I outline the theoretical and methodological underpinnings of developing a “cinematic‐phenomenological research method” for PFCollective in order to illuminate lived experiences of sociocultural oppression for public viewership via cinematography. The roots of cinematic phenomenology are situated within the theoretical framework of liberation psychology, which calls on psychologists to pursue research that can facilitate consciousness‐raising and social justice across society. The paper provides a methodological overview of existential‐hermeneutic phenomenological research and discusses how its research findings can be disseminated to the widespread public in artistic formats. I demonstrate how filmmaking is an appropriate aesthetic language through which to disseminate phenomenological research about lived experiences of oppression, grounding this proposition in phenomenological philosophy and liberation psychology. I outline the procedural steps for conducting cinematic‐phenomenological research to produce phenomenological films about oppressive sociocultural phenomena. The paper concludes with suggestions for academic disciplines to become more interdisciplinary in our collective pursuit for social advocacy and change. 相似文献
65.
Alarge number of studies have demonstrated that participants could be led to report suggested events that were never witnessed (Loftus & Palmer, 1974 ). The present study attempts to explore the quality of such suggested memories. Thirty‐six participants were exposed to a live event of brief duration (20 s). They were then misled about certain aspects of the event. Memory was assessed using a forced‐choice recognition test. The participants were also required to give remember/know/guess responses, together with confidence ratings, and also to indicate how they came to remember the item. Thirty‐six per cent of the suggested memories were “know” responses, while 43% were “remember” responses. Moreover, whenever the participants had a remember experience they always recalled a wealth of visual information. With regards to the confidence level of the suggested memories, it was observed that the suggested memories were as confidently held as accurate memories. The results indicate that suggestions may become integrated with other kinds of information and that these falsely remembered memories are often accompanied with varied kinds of information; predominant among them seems to be visual imagery and past associations. For accurate memories, too, visual imagery was recalled the most, followed by other kinds of information (e.g., past associations, emotional, auditory, and inferential). The study points to the difficulties of differentiating between true recollections of actual events and false recollections of events that did not occur. 相似文献
66.
Gupta M 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2007,28(2):103-120
Evidence-based psychiatry (EBP) has arisen through the application of evidence-based medicine (EBM) to psychiatry. However,
there may be aspects of psychiatric disorders and treatments that do not conform well to the assumptions of EBM. This paper
reviews the ongoing debate about evidence-based psychiatry and investigates the applicability, to psychiatry, of two basic
methodological features of EBM: prognostic homogeneity of clinical trial groups and quantification of trial outcomes. This
paper argues that EBM may not be the best way to pursue psychiatric knowledge given the particular features of psychiatric
disorders and their treatments. As a result, psychiatry may have to develop its own standards for rigour and validity. This
paper concludes that EBM has had a powerful influence on how psychiatry investigates and understands mental disorders. Psychiatry
could influence EBM in return, reshaping it in ways that are more clinically useful and congruent with patients’ needs. 相似文献
67.
Brain mechanisms used to control nonemotional aspects of cognition may be distinct from those regulating responses to emotional
stimuli, with activity of the latter being detrimental to the former. Previous studies have shown that suppression of irrelevant
emotional stimuli produces a largely right-lateralized pattern of frontal brain activation, thus predicting that emotional
stimuli may invoke temporary, lateralized costs to performance on nonemotional cognitive tasks. To test this, we briefly (85 ms)
presented a central, irrelevant, expressive (angry, happy, sad, or fearful) or neutral face 100 ms prior to a letter search
task. The presentation of emotional versus neutral faces slowed subsequent search for targets appearing in the left, but not
the right, hemifield, supporting the notion of a right-lateralized, emotional response mechanism that competes for control
with nonemotional cognitive processes. Presentation of neutral, scrambled, or inverted neutral faces produced no such laterality
effects on visual search response times. 相似文献
68.
Intrinsic persistent spiking mechanisms in medial entorhinal cortex (mEC) neurons may play a role in active maintenance of working memory. However, electrophysiological studies of rat mEC units have primarily focused on spatial modulation. We sought evidence of differential spike rates in the mEC in rats trained on a T-maze, cued spatial delayed response task. Animals begin at the base of the T-maze where a 1-sec white noise and visual light cue are presented on the left or right side of the maze. Rats are rewarded for responding toward the cued direction. In correct trials, we observed decreased spike rates during the delay period, the time interval between cue presentation and reward delivery. Firing-rate histograms show significant decreases during the delay period compared to 5-sec windows from both pre-cue and post-reward periods. We analyzed how running speed and trajectory specificity correlated to spike rate. Twice as many cells were responsive to cue alone compared to running speed. Trajectory specificity did not relate significantly to firing rate. Decreased spike rate may reflect active maintenance in other structures inhibiting mEC. Alternately, the reduction may reflect decreases in background activity during enhanced attention and cholinergic modulation. Lastly, animals often ran through the T-maze choice-point with varying speed. We calculated the spatial posterior probability density from spike rates during these choice-point passes. Slow passes through the choice point were characterized by greater probability of decoding to the reward locations on correct trials compared to quick passes on the maze consistent with similar "look-ahead" properties previously reported in the hippocampus and ventral striatum. 相似文献
69.
G. K. Dey R. Tewari S. Banerjee G. Jyoti S. C. Gupta K. D. Joshi 《Philosophical Magazine Letters》2013,93(6):333-340
It is reported for the first time that a g M y transformation can be induced in a g -stabilized zirconium alloy subjected to shock pressure. The y phase formed in the g matrix has been found to have a plate shape akin to martensitic plates. The lattice correspondence between the g and y structures has been found to be the same as that produced by thermal treatment. The formation of the plate-shaped y phase is explained in terms of a mechanism involving shear on <112> planes of the bcc lattice and the mechanical instability of the g phase. 相似文献
70.
Prahlad Gupta 《Memory (Hove, England)》2013,21(3-4):318-324
An increasing body of evidence suggests that nonword repetition is related to immediate serial memory (e.g., ; ). One possible account of this relationship is that a nonword is processed like a list when it is first encountered. If this is the case, it should be possible to detect serial position effects in repetition of single nonwords. Three experiments tested this prediction. Experiment 1 examined whether there would be syllable serial position primacy and recency effects in repetition of polysyllabic nonwords, and obtained both primacy and recency effects. Experiments 2 and 3 showed that these effects were not due to the controlled duration of the nonwords or the requirements of concurrent articulation or the procedure by which nonwords were created. 相似文献