首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   242篇
  免费   30篇
  国内免费   96篇
  368篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   10篇
  2021年   9篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   28篇
  2012年   15篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   18篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   22篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   26篇
  2004年   18篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
排序方式: 共有368条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
傅维利  刘磊 《心理科学》2006,29(3):724-728
实践智力理论是斯腾伯格等人针对智商研究解释力不足提出的,它是对传统智力理论的有益补充。本文介绍了实践智力理论的核心概念、基本观点以及当前心理学界对之提出的主要质疑,并对该理论的优长与缺憾进行了分析和讨论。  相似文献   
182.
儿童对有关心理特质的自我报告信息的评价   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
傅根跃  丁晓攀 《心理科学》2006,29(2):392-394
本研究通过对63名儿童的访谈来调查儿童对他人有关心理特质的自我报告信息的评价。研究发现,10、11岁儿童比6、7岁儿童对高评价性特质“诚实”、“聪明”、“善良”、“友善”自我报告的信息抱有更大的怀疑;但是在中性特质“忧虑”、“喜欢热闹”、“喜欢红色”、“喜欢咸食”上没有显著差异。结果表明,把自我报告作为有关高评价性特质的信息来源时,高年龄组比低年龄组表现出更大的怀疑,而且这种变化是与儿童自我表现能力的提高有关。  相似文献   
183.
Liu  Yanping  Yu  Lili  Fu  Le  Li  Wenwen  Duan  Ziyi  Reichle  Erik D. 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2019,26(4):1367-1376
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review - Two eye-movement experiments are reported in which a boundary paradigm was used to manipulate the presence versus absence of boundaries for high-frequency and...  相似文献   
184.
采用2×2两因素被试间实验设计探讨初始矛盾态度及危机应对策略对消费者矛盾态度和购买意愿的影响。结果表明:当消费者初始矛盾态度水平较低时更可能受辩解策略的影响,从而使其负面态度和矛盾态度下降; 当消费者初始矛盾态度水平较高时更可能受和解策略的影响,从而使其负面态度和矛盾态度下降; 正面态度/负面态度可以显著正向/负向预测消费者的购买意愿,矛盾态度在其中起调节作用,即矛盾态度水平越高,正面/负面态度与购买意愿之间关系的不确定性越高。  相似文献   
185.
This research sought to integrate the implicit theory approach and the social identity approach to understanding biases in intergroup judgment. The authors hypothesized that a belief in fixed human character would be associated with negative bias and prejudice against a maligned group regardless of the perceiver's social identity. By contrast, a belief in malleable human character would allow the perceiver's social identity to guide intergroup perception, such that a common ingroup identity that includes the maligned group would be associated with less negative bias and prejudice against the maligned group than would an exclusive identity. To test these hypotheses, a correlational study was conducted in the context of the Hong Kong 1997 political transition to examine Hong Kong Chinese's perceptions of Chinese Mainlanders, and an experimental study was conducted in the United States to examine Asian Americans' perception of African Americans. Results from both studies supported the authors' predictions.  相似文献   
186.
幼儿面子谎行为与面子意识研究   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
傅根跃  孙永珍 《心理科学》2003,26(6):1039-1042
本文研究了幼儿的面子谎行为和面子意识。被试为幼儿园大班幼儿60名(平均年龄5.72岁),中班60名(平均年龄4.74岁),小班40名(平均年龄3.75岁),面子谎行为通过幼儿评画行为测得,而面子意识则通过评价说面子谎故事获得。实验结果表明:大班、中班幼儿明显具有面子谎行为,但两年龄组的幼儿面子谎行为的发生率没有显著差异,小班幼儿没有面子谎行为;幼儿面子意识基本上不存在。说明幼儿面子谎行为的发展远远先于面子意识的形成。  相似文献   
187.
Previous research has concluded that presenting individuals with the opportunity to choose is a viable treatment for escape‐maintained behavior. Considering that food refusal behavior has been generally described as avoidant behavior, the present study aimed to evaluate the role of choice between nonpreferred foods on the food consumption and problematic mealtime behavior of two children with food selectivity. Each participant was allowed to choose between four nonpreferred foods in the choice condition and was not allowed to choose in the no‐choice condition. Further, the role of choice as an antecedent manipulation in mediating extinction‐induced responding was evaluated when choice alone was demonstrated to be ineffective in increasing consumption and a nonremoval of the spoon procedure was introduced. Results indicated that providing choices was effective in increasing the food consumption of one participant and was advantageous in decreasing emotional responding when a nonremoval of the spoon procedure was introduced for the second participant. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
188.
十天干十二地支,本是古人用以纪年纪月纪日纪时的文字符号,就跟现在的1234……一样,  相似文献   
189.
This study examined cross-cultural differences and similarities in children's moral understanding of individual- or collective-oriented lies and truths. Seven-, 9-, and 11-year-old Canadian and Chinese children were read stories about story characters facing moral dilemmas about whether to lie or tell the truth to help a group but harm an individual or vice versa. Participants chose to lie or to tell the truth as if they were the character (Experiments 1 and 2) and categorized and evaluated the story characters' truthful and untruthful statements (Experiments 3 and 4). Most children in both cultures labeled lies as lies and truths as truths. The major cultural differences lay in choices and moral evaluations. Chinese children chose lying to help a collective but harm an individual, and they rated it less negatively than lying with opposite consequences. Chinese children rated truth telling to help an individual but harm a group less positively than the alternative. Canadian children did the opposite. These findings suggest that cross-cultural differences in emphasis on groups versus individuals affect children's choices and moral judgments about truth and deception.  相似文献   
190.
原型理论视野中的亲社会行为研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
亲社会行为是伦理学、心理学、社会学等学科研究的中心议题之一。学界对于什么是亲社会行为、如何对亲社会行为实施测评等一系列问题依然没有达成统一的认识。该文从概念原型理论视角出发,分析了将原型理论纳入青少年亲社会行为研究的可行性,利用原型理论探讨了青少年对亲社会行为的概念表征问题,并提出根据青少年的亲社会行为的概念结构,建构亲社会行为测评体系。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号