全文获取类型
收费全文 | 417篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
431篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 38篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有431条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
214.
Dorothy L. Espelage Alexandra L. Quittner Roberta Sherman Ron Thompson 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2000,22(3):271-297
The current study examined the psychometric properties of the Anorexia and Bulimia Problem Inventory (ABPI; Eason, 1983) in women with and without diagnosed eating disorders. The ABPI was initially constructed in accordance with the Behavior-Analytic model of instrument development (Goldfried & D'Zurilla, 1969). In this investigation, the ABPI was refined to consist of 38 audiotaped problematic situations, including those related to eating and weight, academic, family, and interpersonal relationship issues, and scoring criteria to rate the effectiveness of responses. Convergent and discriminant validity were established between ABPI-R scores and the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983) scales. As evidence of disciminative validity women with eating disorders received less ratings indicating less effective problem-solving on the ABPI-R scales than women without eating disorders. A significant increase in effective coping as measured by the ABPI-R was found for women following an 8-week outpatient eating disorders treatment program. Potential uses of this measure in research and clinical practice are discussed. 相似文献
215.
216.
The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model 总被引:2,自引:3,他引:2
下载免费PDF全文

Donald M. Baer Robert F. Peterson James A. Sherman 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(5):405-416
This research demonstrated some of the conditions under which retarded children can be taught to imitate the actions of adults. Before the experiment, the subjects were without spontaneous imitative behavior, either vocal or motor. Each subject was taught, with food as reinforcement, a series of responses identical to responses demonstrated by an experimenter; i.e., each response was reinforced only if it was identical to a prior demonstration by an experimenter. Initially, intensive shaping was required to establish matching responses by the subjects. In the course of acquiring a variety of such responses, the subjects' probability of immediate imitation of each new demonstration, before direct training, greatly increased. Later in the study, certain new imitations, even though perfect, were never reinforced; yet as long as some imitative responses were reinforced, all remained at high strength. This imitativeness was then used to establish initial verbal repertoires in two subjects. 相似文献
217.
218.
John M. Quinn James A. Sherman Jan B. Sheldon Liane M. Quinn Alan E. Harchik 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):401-413
This study evaluated whether behaviors often taught as part of social skills training are judged favorably by others. Community judges evaluated the performances of people in various situations requiring one of three social skills: following instructions, accepting criticism, and negotiating to resolve conflicts. These skills were displayed in videotaped scenes by actors with and without mental retardation who acted out roles that had different types of authority relationships, and when different components or clusters of behavior (nonverbal, specific verbal, or general verbal behaviors) were performed well or poorly. The highest ratings by judges were of videotaped scenes that depicted correct use of all behaviors, regardless of which skill was being examined, whether or not the actor had mental retardation, or what the relationship was between the two actors. The lowest ratings were of videotaped scenes that depicted poor performance of all behaviors, and intermediate ratings were obtained when only some of the behaviors were performed poorly. These results, as well as the verbal responses of judges to questions, indicated that the different behaviors commonly used in teaching the skills of following instructions, accepting criticism, and negotiating are relevant to judgment of social performance, and are likely to be reinforced and maintained by social contingencies. 相似文献
219.
Julia A. Sherman 《Psychology of women quarterly》1982,6(4):428-444
Interviews were conducted regarding attitudes toward sex role and mathematics with three cognitively equated groups of girls who had enrolled in different amounts of theoretical (college-track) math in high school ( n =87). Girls who took the least math were less ambitious and in some ways, more independent. Girls who took four years of theoretical math had more pleasant earliest memories of math, fewer negative experiences with teachers, but more conflict between sex role and achievement. Results continue to underscore contradictions in the socialization of girls. 相似文献
220.
Maryanne Garry Charles G. Manning Elizabeth F. Loftus Steven J. Sherman 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》1996,3(2):208-214
Counterfactual imaginings are known to have far-reaching implications. In the present experiment, we ask if imagining events from one’s past can affect memory for childhood events. We draw on the social psychology literature showing that imagining a future event increases the subjective likelihood that the event will occur. The concepts of cognitive availability and the source-monitoring framework provide reasons to expect that imagination may inflate confidence that a childhood event occurred. However, people routinely produce myriad counterfactual imaginings (i.e., daydreams and fantasies) but usually do not confuse them with past experiences. To determine the effects of imagining a childhood event, we pretested subjects on how confident they were that a number of childhood events had happened, asked them to imagine some of those events, and then gathered new confidence measures. For each of the target items, imagination inflated confidence that the event had occurred in childhood. We discuss implications for situations in which imagination is used as an aid in searching for presumably lost memories. 相似文献