首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   87759篇
  免费   3414篇
  国内免费   27篇
  2019年   1055篇
  2018年   1434篇
  2017年   1431篇
  2016年   1558篇
  2015年   1123篇
  2014年   1348篇
  2013年   7316篇
  2012年   2441篇
  2011年   2611篇
  2010年   1638篇
  2009年   1606篇
  2008年   2382篇
  2007年   2313篇
  2006年   2133篇
  2005年   1854篇
  2004年   1738篇
  2003年   1692篇
  2002年   1765篇
  2001年   2764篇
  2000年   2690篇
  1999年   2070篇
  1998年   977篇
  1997年   889篇
  1996年   1009篇
  1995年   934篇
  1994年   886篇
  1993年   900篇
  1992年   1879篇
  1991年   1723篇
  1990年   1710篇
  1989年   1549篇
  1988年   1515篇
  1987年   1499篇
  1986年   1530篇
  1985年   1546篇
  1984年   1343篇
  1983年   1180篇
  1981年   895篇
  1979年   1352篇
  1978年   996篇
  1975年   1177篇
  1974年   1216篇
  1973年   1337篇
  1972年   1201篇
  1971年   1078篇
  1970年   984篇
  1969年   995篇
  1968年   1252篇
  1967年   1097篇
  1966年   1000篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
71.
72.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
73.
74.
75.
Shaw  Jamie 《Synthese》2021,198(1):419-449

To anyone vaguely aware of Feyerabend, the title of this paper would appear as an oxymoron. For Feyerabend, it is often thought, science is an anarchic practice with no discernible structure. Against this trend, I elaborate the groundwork that Feyerabend has provided for the beginnings of an approach to organizing scientific research. Specifically, I argue that Feyerabend’s pluralism, once suitably modified, provides a plausible account of how to organize science. These modifications come from C.S. Peirce’s account of the economics of theory pursuit, which has since been corroborated by empirical findings in the social sciences. I go on to contrast this approach with the conception of a ‘well-ordered science’ as outlined by Kitcher (Science, truth, and democracy, Oxford University Press, New York, 2001), Cartwright (Philos Sci 73(5):981–990, 2006), which rests on the assumption that we can predict the content of future research. I show how Feyerabend has already given us reasons to think that this model is much more limited than it is usually understood. I conclude by showing how models of resource allocation, specifically those of Kitcher (J Philos 87:5–22, 1990), Strevens (J Philos 100(2):55–79, 2003) and Weisberg and Muldoon (Philos Sci 76(2):225–252, 2009), unwittingly make use of this problematic assumption. I conclude by outlining a proposed model of resource allocation where funding is determined by lottery and briefly examining the extent to which it is compatible with the position defended in this paper.

  相似文献   
76.
An electronic apparatus using infrared beams for monitoring the movements of individual bees under dark conditions is described. The searching behavior of workers in an arena was monitored over 2-h periods. Mean ambulatory velocity for one bee over a distance of 100 mm was 45.6±1.51 mm/sec (n=45). Thigmokinesis and temporal activity patterns are illustrated.  相似文献   
77.
78.
79.
80.
This paper examines college students' attributions about suicide as a function of gender and context (athletic vs. relationship failure). Results indicated that male athletes who suicided were considered more emotionally well-adjusted compared to males who suicided because of a relationship failure and all females. Male and female athletes who suicided were seen as more competent and less distressed compared to those who suffered a failed relationship. Females were also seen as more distressed compared to males. These results are discussed in terms of a positive bias toward athletes in our society as well as gender stereotypes. Implications of these findings as well as methodological limitations are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号