全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2474篇 |
免费 | 175篇 |
国内免费 | 336篇 |
专业分类
2985篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 24篇 |
2022年 | 40篇 |
2021年 | 46篇 |
2020年 | 70篇 |
2019年 | 79篇 |
2018年 | 85篇 |
2017年 | 114篇 |
2016年 | 118篇 |
2015年 | 69篇 |
2014年 | 87篇 |
2013年 | 311篇 |
2012年 | 134篇 |
2011年 | 138篇 |
2010年 | 109篇 |
2009年 | 86篇 |
2008年 | 125篇 |
2007年 | 130篇 |
2006年 | 150篇 |
2005年 | 112篇 |
2004年 | 95篇 |
2003年 | 90篇 |
2002年 | 72篇 |
2001年 | 57篇 |
2000年 | 62篇 |
1999年 | 42篇 |
1998年 | 40篇 |
1997年 | 45篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2985条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
BLAMING THE PERPETRATOR Language that Distorts Reality in Newspaper Articles on Men Battering Women 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
In an earlier article, Lamb (1991) showed that journal authors, when writing about men battering women, wrote in a way that avoided assigning responsibility to men as perpetrators, and that this kind of writing was more common among male authors as well as female authors who wrote with men. This study examines first whether this kind of writing occurs in newspaper articles on men battering women, and whether two problematic styles of writing have an effect on the reader. Three versions of a newspaper article were developed to differentiate active voice, passive voice, and writing that implies shared responsibility for a man's violence. One hundred and eighty subjects read one of the three versions and endorsed one of five possible punishments for the man in the story who had been violent. Results showed that subjects did not differ in their selection of punishments for the active voice versus the passive voice version, but were much more lenient towards the man after reading the shared responsibility version. 相似文献
22.
23.
Sharon A. Borthwick-Duffy Ph.D. David S. Palmer Ph.D. Kathleen L. Lane M.A. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1996,6(3):311-329
Recent studies have documented the increasing trend among school districts toward adopting full inclusion as the predominant model for serving students who qualify for special education services. Although these data may suggest that a paradigm shift has already taken place and that a consensus of opinion has been achieved, several organizations representing students with specific disabilities continue to support a continuum of placement options. This paper challenges a one size fits all approach to special education, and highlights the importance of considering the individual characteristics of students. The relevance of research on full inclusion, as well as methodological issues that limit generalizability of findings related to student outcomes, are discussed. The radical perspective of full inclusion is rejected in favor of an approach that emphasizes the complexities of placement decisions. 相似文献
24.
Aleidine J. Moeller Ph.D. Sharon Ishii-Jordan Ph.D. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1996,6(3):293-310
Students with special needs must have the opportunity to learn in inclusive classrooms. However, this requires that classroom teachers believe that (a) students can learn in spite of any negative environmental influences in their lives, and (b) they personally have the ability to reach any student. Securing this teaching efficacy for teachers in inclusive classroom settings requires paradigm shifts in preservice and inservice teacher development programs with the precept that all students can learn and that we can assist them. The authors, one a regular and the other a special education teacher educator, collaborate to explore the issue of how best to provide and maintain a positive personal teaching efficacy among regular teacher education candidates who are faced with the challenges of having students with varying levels of ability and behavioral responses in their classrooms. 相似文献
25.
26.
Sharon L. James 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1978,7(4):307-317
Twenty-one children between the ages of 46 and 52 years made a speaker doll address other dolls that represented an adult, a peer, and a younger listener in command and request situations. Analyses of the politeness of the elicited directives revealed that the effect of the listener's age was greatest in the command situations,with the adult listener receiving the politest directives, followed by the peer and the younger child, respectively. The listener age effect diminished in the request situations, where the child had to ask a favor of the listener. In these situations, the children were very polite to all three listeners. The results are discussed in terms of status relationships between the speaker and the listener.This article is taken from a doctoral dissertation completed at the University of Wisconsin, Madison, under the direction of D. K. Vetter. 相似文献
27.
一 实验心理学已有悠久的历史,它建立于前一世纪的中叶,采用自然科学的方法研究心理学问题,在使心理学成为一门独立的科学上起了重要作用。但心理学被介绍到中国来还是在清朝末期,最早的一个心理学实验室是在1917年北京大学哲学系建立的。解放前的旧中国已有七、八个高等学校设置了心理学系,除了讲授普通心理学课程外,还开设 相似文献
28.
The experimental analysis of behavior has grown enormously in Latin America within the last decade. This paper offers an updating revision of suggested Spanish translations of operant expressions. 相似文献
29.
Eli Osman Pi -Fong Lin Tzuo Huan -Yuan Tzuo 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1981,30(4):372-374
The threshold of a brief signal temporally centered in a noise masker first increases and then decreases as the masker is increased in duration. We discuss two explanations for this “over-shoot” phenomenon which consider the role of peaks of an exponentially weighted sliding integrator. One such explanation takes into account the finding that the steepness of the psychometric function continues to increase monotonically as its location moves first right and then left. 相似文献
30.
Mary Ann Balaz Sharon Capra Phillipe Hartl Ralph R. Miller 《Learning and motivation》1981,12(4):383-397
In two experiments, water-deprived rats were exposed to tone-footshock pairings in a particular conditioning context. In Experiment 1, the training parameters were selected on the basis of parametric pilot data so as not to produce direct associations between the conditioning context and footshock. Despite independent evidence of no appreciable direct association between the conditioning context and footshock, animals tested in the conditioning context exhibited more lick suppression to the tone than other animals tested with the tone outside of the conditioning context. Thus, the conditioning context potentiated suppression to the tone in the absence of appreciable fear of the conditioning context. In Experiment 2, training parameters were used that permitted the formation of direct associations between the conditioning context and footshock; however, for half the animals, these associations to the context were then extinguished. With the tone absent, a comparison of extinguished animals tested either in the conditioning context or outside the conditioning context detected no differences in suppression, demonstrating the effectiveness of the extinction manipulation. However, other animals also subjected to extinction of direct context-footshock associations displayed greater suppression to the tone within the conditioning context than comparable animals tested outside the context. Collectively the data indicate that the superior retention-test performance seen within the training context can arise both from the commonly assumed direct associations between the training context and the unconditioned stimulus and from the potentiation by the training context of the associations between the nominal conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. 相似文献