全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1337篇 |
免费 | 13篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1351篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 32篇 |
2019年 | 48篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 58篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 134篇 |
2012年 | 57篇 |
2011年 | 63篇 |
2010年 | 51篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 67篇 |
2005年 | 48篇 |
2004年 | 39篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 33篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1351条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
Sharon Nelson-Le Gall 《Developmental Review》1981,1(3):224-246
Traditional approaches to the study of young children's behavior in helping relationships are examined and criticized as inadequate because they have failed to represent the child's perspective from the role of “active helpee” (i.e., help-seeker in such relationships). By failing to look at helping from the perspective of the one who seeks help, researchers have neglected to pursue an important lead in understanding why some children are able to learn and progress independently when confronted with the same obstacles that serve to defeat other children. This article focuses on instrumental help-seeking defined as an active, complex social-cognitive activity that is essential to learning and achievement. In the first sections of this article, it is argued that instrumental help-seeking can be formally distinguished from passive dependency as well as from the actual giving and receiving of help. In following sections, a heuristic model of the help-seeking process is offered, prior research relevant to the model is reviewed, and ideas for research on help-seeking in children are suggested within the framework of this model. 相似文献
882.
883.
884.
Chloe Gordon Sharon Tindall‐Ford Shirley Agostinho Fred Paas 《Applied cognitive psychology》2016,30(1):1-9
Instructor‐managed physical integration of mutually dependent, but spatially separated materials, is an effective way to overcome negative effects of split‐attention on learning. This study examined whether teaching students to self‐manage split‐attention materials would be effective for learning. Seventy‐eight primary‐school students learned about the water cycle, either by studying split‐attention examples, integrated examples or self‐managed split‐attention examples. It was hypothesised that students who study instructor‐integrated materials and students who study self‐integrated materials would outperform students who study split‐attention materials. The results showed that students learned more from instructor‐integrated materials than from split‐attention materials, thereby confirming the split‐attention effect. The implications for future research on self‐management are discussed.Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
885.
Sarah Ewens Aldert Vrij Sharon Leal Samantha Mann Eunkyung Jo Alla Shaboltas Maria Ivanova Juliana Granskaya Kate Houston 《Applied cognitive psychology》2016,30(6):854-862
We examined how the presence of an interpreter during an interview affects eliciting information and cues to deceit, whilst using a method that encourages interviewees to provide more detail (model statement, MS). Sixty native English speakers were interviewed in English, and 186 non‐native English speakers were interviewed in English or through an interpreter. Interviewees either lied or told the truth about a mock security meeting, which they reported twice: in an initial free recall and after listening to the MS. The MS resulted in the native English speakers and those interviewed with an interpreter providing more reminiscences (additional detail) than the non‐native English speakers interviewed without an interpreter. As a result, those interviewed through an interpreter provided more detail than the non‐native English speakers, but only after the MS. Native English participants were most detailed in both recalls. No difference was found in the amount of reminiscences provided by liars and truth tellers. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
886.
The present experiment examined whether people could be deterred from lying in an online insurance claim setting. A total of 96 participants were asked to submit a theft insurance claim. Reflecting real life, submitting a claim that went beyond the actual costs of the stolen items was associated with advantages and disadvantages. Two deterrence factors were introduced: asking claimants to provide evidence that they actually owned the stolen items (Evidence Instruction, often used by insurers) and asking participants to read out before starting to submit the claim that they will be truthful (Honesty Statement, not often used by insurers). We also examined at what stage of the interview claimants embedded their lies in their otherwise truthful stories. The honesty statement but not the evidence instruction made claimants more honest, and participants lied more as the interview progressed. © 2016 The Authors Applied Cognitive Psychology Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 相似文献
887.
To study value change, this research presents an intervention with multiple exercises designed to instigate change through both effortful and automatic routes. Aiming to increase the importance attributed to benevolence values, which reflect the motivation to help and care for others, the intervention combines three mechanisms for value change (self‐persuasion, consistency‐maintenance, and priming). In three experiments, 142 undergraduates (67% male, ages 19–26) participated in an intervention emphasizing the importance of either helping others (benevolence condition) or recognizing flexibility in personality (control condition). We measured the importance of benevolence values before and after the task. In Experiment 1, the intervention increased U.S. participants' benevolence values. In Experiment 2, we replicated these effects in a different culture (Israel) and also showed that by enhancing benevolence values, the intervention increased participants' willingness to volunteer to help others. Experiment 3 showed that the increases in the importance of benevolence values lasted at least 4 weeks. Our results provide evidence that value change does not require fictitious feedback or information about social norms, but can occur through a 30‐min intervention that evokes both effortful and automatic processes. 相似文献
888.
889.
Daniel B. Turban Felissa K. Lee Serge P. da Motta Veiga Dana L. Haggard Sharon Y. Wu 《Personnel Psychology》2013,66(2):483-514
In this study we developed and tested a self‐regulatory model of trait affect in job search. Specifically, we theorized that trait positive and negative affect would influence both motivation control and procrastination, and these mediating variables would, in turn, influence job search outcomes through job search intensity. Using longitudinal data from 245 graduating students who were searching for a full‐time position, we found that positive, but not negative, affect influenced the self‐regulatory variables of motivation control and procrastination, which in turn influenced the job search outcomes. Procrastination had direct effects on the number of first interviews, controlling for job search intensity, and on the number of second interviews, controlling for first interviews, suggesting the importance of timeliness of job search activities. We discuss the implications of such results for understanding the role of affect and self‐regulation in the job search process and for measuring the quality as well as quantity (i.e., intensity) of job search tactics. 相似文献
890.
Catherine Quinn Ian Rees Jones Anthony Martyr Sharon M. Nelis Robin G. Morris Linda Clare 《Psychology & health》2013,28(10):1214-1230
AbstractObjective: Informal caregivers of people with dementia develop their own beliefs about the condition, referred to as Dementia Representations (DRs), as they try to make sense of the changes they are observing. The first aim of this study was to provide a profile of the types of DRs held by caregivers. The second aim was to examine the impact of caregivers’ DRs on their well-being, satisfaction with life (SwL) and caregiving stress.Methods: Participants were 1264 informal caregivers of people in the mild-to-moderate stages of dementia from time-point 1 of the IDEAL cohort study.Measures: DRs were measured using questionnaire items covering: Identity, Cause, Control, and Timeline.Results: Almost half (49.2%) of caregivers used a diagnostic term to describe the person’s condition, although 93.4% of caregivers stated they were aware of the diagnosis. Higher well-being, SwL, and lower caregiving stress were associated with the use of an identity term relating to specific symptoms of dementia, attributing the cause to ageing or not knowing the cause, and believing the condition would stay the same. Lower well-being, SwL, and higher caregiving stress were associated with believing there was little that could be done to control the effects of the condition.Conclusion: Healthcare professionals should assess and gain an understanding of caregivers’ DRs in order to provide more tailored information and support. 相似文献