全文获取类型
收费全文 | 712篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
739篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 74篇 |
2012年 | 37篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有739条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Skov-Rackette SI Shettleworth SJ 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2005,31(2):142-154
Six experiments using habituation of exploratory behavior tested whether disoriented rats foraging in a large arena encode the shapes of arrays of objects. Rats did not respond to changes in position of a single object, but they responded to a change in object color and to a change in position of 1 object in a square array, as in previous research (e.g., C. Thinus-Blanc et al., 1987). Rats also responded to an expansion of a square array, suggesting that they encoded sets of interobject distances rather than overall shape. In Experiments 4-6, rats did not respond to changes in sense of a triangular array that maintained interobject distances and angles. Shapes of object arrays are encoded differently from shapes of enclosures. 相似文献
42.
Past research on the accuracy of personality judgments has largely focused on person perception scenarios that are public in nature (e.g., face-to-face interactions, personal websites). This study investigated the accuracy of personality judgments on the basis of highly private information: a person’s natural stream of thought. Nine naïve judges rated the personality of 90 targets on the basis of their 20-min stream-of-consciousness essays. Judges’ level of accuracy was significant and substantial for all Big Five dimensions. The substantial and relatively uniform accuracy across all Big Five dimensions suggests that a person’s moment-to-moment thoughts provide good information for the accurate judgment of personality in general rather than specific diagnostic information for the accurate judgment of private traits such as Neuroticism. 相似文献
43.
Actors have feelings too: An examination of justice climate effects on the psychological well‐being of organizational authority figures 下载免费PDF全文
44.
Shannon B. Wanless 《Research in human development》2016,13(1):1-5
Human development is characterized by individual agency to create change, to a greater or lesser extent depending on contextual circumstances. This special issue focuses on psychological safety as a construct that may help developmentalists understand the way that individual and contextual factors come together to limit or support agency to shape one’s development. This introduction to the issue on psychological safety frames this construct and the included articles through the lens of relational developmental systems theory. Specifically, authors explore psychological safety as a construct at the intersection of individual context. They use multiple methods and draw across disciplines to explore psychological safety in adolescence, during the transition to parenthood, in the workplace, and at the organizational level. Overall, findings suggest that additional research and practical focus on psychological safety may increase the likelihood that individuals may enact their agency to engage in opportunities and experiences that support human development. 相似文献
45.
46.
Two studies tested the prediction that group identification (importance of the group in the self-concept) moderates the impact of perceived discrimination on self-evaluative emotions (depression and self-esteem). In Study 1, women low in gender identification experienced less depressed emotion and higher self-esteem if a negative evaluation was due to sexism than when it was not. The self-evaluative emotions of women high in gender identification were not buffered by attributions to sexism. In Study 2, ethnic identification and depressed emotions were positively related when Latino-Americans read about pervasive prejudice against the ingroup but were negatively related when they read about prejudice against an outgroup. Both studies demonstrated that for highly group identified individuals, prejudice against the ingroup is a threat against the self. Thus, the self-protective strategy of attributing negative feedback to discrimination may be primarily effective for individuals who do not consider the group a central aspect of self. 相似文献
47.
Williamson HC Ju X Bradbury TN Karney BR Fang X Liu X 《Journal of family psychology》2012,26(3):308-315
Most research on couple communication patterns comes from North America and Europe and suggests cross-cultural universality in effects, but emerging studies suggest that couple communication takes different forms depending on the cultural context in which it occurs. The current study addressed this discrepancy by comparing the observed social support behaviors of 50 newlywed American couples and 41 newlywed Mainland Chinese couples, first on mean levels of positivity and negativity and second on behavior-satisfaction associations. Consistent with predictions derived from observational work by Tsai and Levenson (1997), Chinese couples were observed displaying significantly more negative behavior than American couples, even after controlling for relationship satisfaction; the 2 groups did not differ in observed positive behaviors. Tests of the moderating role of culture on behavior-satisfaction associations showed that positivity was significantly related to relationship satisfaction only for American husbands, whereas negativity was significantly associated with relationship satisfaction only for Chinese husbands. We speculate that cultural contexts may influence the display and evaluation of behavior in intimate relationships, suggesting the need for caution when generalizing models and associated interventions to non-Western couples. 相似文献
48.
John H. Porcerelli William Murdoch Pierre Morris Shannon Fowler 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2014,21(3):291-296
This study assessed the validity of the Patient–Doctor Relationship Questionnaire-9 (PDRQ-9) in a primary care sample (N = 180). Convergent validity was assessed through a correlation between the patient-rated PDRQ-9 and the physician-rated Difficult Doctor Patient Relationship Questionnaire-10 (DDPRQ-10). Discriminant validity was assessed through correlations between the PDRQ-9 and patient age, patient- and physician-reported health and psychological distress. To determine if the PDRQ-9 could discriminate between groups, patient PDRQ-9 ratings were compared between patients who were treated by faculty physicians versus those who were treated by residents. An exploratory factor analysis confirmed that the PDRQ-9 was made up of a single factor. The PDRQ-9 scale was internally consistent (α = .96) and significantly and negatively correlated with the DDPRQ-10 (r = ?.22, p = .003) and was not significantly correlated with patient age, health, or psychological distress. PDRQ-9 ratings were statistically greater in patients who were treated by faculty physicians than those who were treated by residents (p = .01). This study provides additional support for the reliability and validity of the PDRQ-9 as a measure of the doctor–patient relationship in a primary care sample. 相似文献
49.
Shannon E. Sauer Ruth A. Baer 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2009,31(4):378-386
The authors examined the relationships between change-based and acceptance-based strategies for responding to negative internal
experience (thoughts and emotions) and levels of psychological symptoms and well-being. A large sample of undergraduate students
completed measures of their general frequency of experiencing negative affect and intrusive thoughts, their typical ways of
responding when these experiences occur, and their levels of psychological symptoms and well-being. Correlational analyses
showed that most of the identified ways of responding to negative internal experiences were significantly related to psychological
symptoms and well-being, even after accounting for the general frequency of experiencing unwanted thoughts and emotions. Regression
analyses suggested that change-based ways of responding add little or no incremental variance over acceptance-based strategies
in accounting for lower symptom levels and greater well-being. 相似文献
50.
Internalization of the thin ideal has been thought to occur disproportionately among Caucasian, upper-class girls and women. The current research challenges this view by assessing thin-ideal internalization among low-income ethnic minority girls (N = 48). This research identifies differences between low-income ethnic minority adolescent girls who highly internalize the thin ideal (HI) and those who have lower internalization of thinness (LI). Results indicate that HI and LI groups do not differ based on ethnicity or weight categorization. Body satisfaction, competence, and perceived sociocultural pressure were analyzed as individual differences between HI versus LI participants. Those in the LI group have higher athletic competence and body satisfaction ratings for weight compared to the HI group. By identifying differences in thin-ideal internalization in this understudied population, factors that may protect someone from endorsing the thin ideal may be discovered. 相似文献