全文获取类型
收费全文 | 386篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
392篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 42篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 77篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有392条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
A common belief in the study of short-term memory is that the verbal trace decays around two seconds after it is encoded. This belief is typically assumed to follow from the finding that in immediate serial recall, the time required to rapidly articulate a span-length list is around two seconds. Empirically, this belief is in opposition to a broad set of findings across a number of domains that establish mean decay times to be longer than two seconds. Theoretically, the available computational and mathematical models of immediate serial recall do not address this issue directly, because they typically rely on other mechanisms in addition to decay to account for forgetting. As such, they may show that decay times can be longer than two seconds, but they fail to show that they cannot be as short as two seconds. We address the issue directly and set a lower bound on mean trace decay times, even under the limiting assumption that all forgetting is due to trace decay. We do this by presenting a simple item-based model of trace decay that allows us to estimate values of mean trace duration. For a set of words whose span-length lists can be rapidly articulated in about two seconds, the model offers a conservative estimate for their mean decay times of around four seconds. Both the experimental and theoretical evidence show that items in verbal working memory decay considerably slower than the two-second decay hypothesis claims. 相似文献
152.
Linford A. Williams Shane C. Quinonez Wendy R. Uhlmann 《Journal of genetic counseling》2017,26(5):894-901
Mandibulofacial dysostosis with microcephaly (MFDM) is a rare autosomal dominant condition that was first described in 2006. The causative gene, EFTUD2, identified in 2012. We report on a family that initially presented to a pediatric genetics clinic in the 1980s for evaluation of multiple congenital anomalies. Re-evaluation of one member thirty years later resulted in a phenotypic and molecularly confirmed diagnosis of MFDM. This family’s clinical histories and the novel EFTUD2 variant identified, c.1297_1298delAT (p.Met433Valfs*17), add to the literature about MFDM. This case presented several genetic counseling challenges and highlights that “the patient” can be multiple family members. We discuss testing considerations for an unknown disorder complicated by the time constraint of the patient’s daughter’s pregnancy and how the diagnosis changed previously provided recurrence risks. Of note, 1) the 1980s clinic visit letters provided critical information about affected family members and 2) the patient’s husband’s internet search of his wife’s clinical features also yielded the MFDM diagnosis, illustrating the power of the internet in the hands of patients. Ultimately, this case emphasizes the importance of re-evaluation given advances in genetics and the value of a genetic diagnosis for both patient care and risk determination for family members. 相似文献
153.
Shane Sharp 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2012,51(2):257-265
Social actors use prayer utterances (e.g., statements such as “prayed over,”“prayed about,” or “spent time in prayer”) as aligning actions that justify problematic or questionable courses of action and prevent possible negative characterizations. The prayer utterance functions as an aligning action in American society because of Americans’ beliefs and actions concerning the efficacy of prayer in solving problems and because prayer is viewed as a sign of personal morality and trustworthiness. Three examples of individuals who use the prayer utterance as an aligning action serve as illustrations of this argument (Oprah Winfrey, Alabama State Supreme Court Justice Sue Bell Cobb, and President Barack Obama). Scholars should investigate the uses and functions of prayer utterances in social life and should also investigate the possible uses of other religious talk as aligning actions. 相似文献
154.
Differentiating corporal punishment from physical abuse in the prediction of lifetime aggression
下载免费PDF全文

Alan R King Abrianna Ratzak Sage Ballantyne Shane Knutson Tiffany D. Russell Colton R. Pogalz Cody M. Breen 《Aggressive behavior》2018,44(3):306-315
155.
156.
In a series of four experiments, we evaluated observers' abilities to perceive and discriminate ordinal depth relationships between separated local surface regions for objects depicted by static, deforming, and disparate boundary contours or silhouettes. Comparisons were also made between judgments made for silhouettes and for objects defined by surface texture, which permits judgment based on conventional static texture gradients, conventional stereopsis, and conventional structure-from-motion. In all the experiments, the observers were able to detect, with relatively high precision, ordinal depth relationships, an aspect of local three-dimensional (3-D) structure, from boundary contours or silhouettes. The results of the experiments clearly demonstrate that the static, disparate, and deforming boundary contours of solid objects are perceptually important optical sources of information about 3-D shape. Other factors that were found to affect performance were the amount of separation between the local surface regions, the proximity or closeness of the regions to the boundary contour itself, and for the conditions with deforming contours, the overall magnitude of the boundary deformation. 相似文献
157.
Callanan N 《Journal of genetic counseling》2006,15(2):73-75
This Presidential Address was given at the annual education conference of the National Society of Genetic Counselors in Los Angeles, California, USA on November 14, 2005 相似文献
158.
159.
Unique developmental crises in old age may lead to social withdrawal that negatively affects the individual and his/her marital and family relationships. Without an awareness of these aging dynamics, the therapist can inadvertently perpetuate these distancing behaviors which can exacerbate marital and family conflict. This paper discusses certain developmental crises which can result in increased isolation for the older adult, and suggests therapeutic interventions to facilitate increased closeness among family members. 相似文献
160.
Theodore L. Gessner Jennifer A. O’Connor Timothy C. Clifton Mary Shane Connelly Michael D. Mumford 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1993,12(3):236-259
The present study tested a model of moral belief development based on Erikson’s (1963) and McAdams’ (1989) theories of personality
development. The sequence of moral belief development is beliefs about outcomes, outcome certainty, beliefs about humanity,
and social concern. The developmental antecedents of these beliefs were measured using 13 rationally constructed scales based
on background data items. The belief and developmental scales were given to a sample of 246 undergraduates. Using a series
of hierarchical blocked regressions, we found a pattern among the beliefs that supported a stage or contingent sequence model
of belief development. The results suggest that a supportive, conventional and consistent family environment is a major antecedent
of beliefs about outcomes and outcome certainty. Adjustment to the demands of adult culture rather than peer culture is the
major antecedent of belief about humanity and social concern.
Parts of this research were supported by a grant from the U.S. Office of Naval Research (Contract No. N00014-91-J-4167), Michael
D. Mumford and Theodore L. Gessner, Principal Investigators. 相似文献