全文获取类型
收费全文 | 267篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
269篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 23篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 9篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1937年 | 1篇 |
1936年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有269条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Christian Frings Iring Koch Birte Moeller 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2017,79(8):2449-2459
Involuntary retrieval of previous stimulus–response episodes is a centerpiece of many theories of priming, episodic binding, and action control. Typically it is assumed that by repeating a stimulus from trial n–1 to trial n, involuntary retrieval is triggered in a nearly automatic fashion, facilitating (or interfering with) the to-be-executed action. Here we argue that changes in the offline context weaken the involuntary retrieval of previous episodes (the offline context is defined to be the information presented before or after the focal stimulus). In four conditions differing in cue modality and target modality, retrieval was diminished if participants changed the target selection criterion (as indicated by a cue presented before the selection took place) while they still performed the same task. Thus, solely through changes in the offline context (cue or selection criterion), involuntary retrieval can be weakened in an effective way. 相似文献
182.
The present study examined cross-modal selective attention using a task-switching paradigm. In a series of experiments, we
presented lateralized visual and auditory stimuli simultaneously and asked participants to make a spatial decision according
to either the visual or the auditory stimulus. We observed consistent cross-modal interference in the form of a spatial congruence
effect. This effect was asymmetrical, with higher costs when responding to auditory than to visual stimuli. Furthermore, we
found stimulus-modality-shift costs, indicating a persisting attentional bias towards the attended stimulus modality. We discuss
our findings with respect to visual dominance, directed-attention accounts, and the modality-appropriateness hypothesis. 相似文献
183.
Pfordresher PQ Keller PE Koch I Palmer C Yildirim E 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2011,18(3):544-549
The present research addressed whether auditory feedback associated with a learned action sequence can activate the action
representation of that sequence. Nonpianist participants learned to perform two melodies at a piano keyboard repeatedly during
a trial. The participants heard feedback either from the melody they were performing (normal feedback) or from the other learned
melody (termed alternate feedback). An additional tone functioned as an instruction cue to either switch melodies or continue the current melody.
Following the instruction cues, participants typically paused just before switching, and paused similarly during trials with
a continue cue. Participants paused longer after a continue cue when they experienced alternate rather than normal feedback.
This effect was specific to instruction cues positioned at weak metrical accents—positions at which participants were less
likely to switch overall. Feedback did not influence timing on switch trials. These findings indicate that influences of auditory
feedback can activate learned action sequences, leading to longer latencies associated with cue evaluation. 相似文献
184.
In task switching, response repetitions typically lead to performance benefits for task repetitions but costs for task switches. We examined whether this cost–benefit pattern is affected by response discriminability (RD), varying (a) the anatomical response separation (within-hand vs. between-hand responses) and (b) the spatial separation (close vs. far response keys). We assumed that anatomical RD increases response competition generally, whereas spatial RD increases the salience of left–right coding and thus facilitates response selection. In two experiments, we found that spatial RD increased the response-repetition costs in task switches but similarly decreased the response-repetition benefit in task repetitions. The effect of spatial RD was response-specific but did not interact with task switching. This data pattern is consistent with a recent account that proposed that facilitated response selection increases response “self-inhibition” after response execution. In contrast, the influence of anatomical RD primarily consisted of an overall increase of reaction-time level in all conditions, whereas error rates decreased, suggesting a general shift in response criterion. Taken together, the data suggest that a self-inhibition mechanism on the level of motor response codes contributes to response-repetition effects in task switching, which is possibly independent of task-specific mechanisms of strengthening of associations. 相似文献
185.
Visual rivalry has been extensively characterized in the literature. It is thought to require spatial conflict between overlapping visual presentations, even in studies that have found nonspatial (i.e., nonretinal) influences on rivalry. Unexpectedly, we identified visual rivalry in the complete absence of spatial conflict. Participants experienced visual rivalry when we placed a nonambiguous motion stimulus in a nonspatial (in our case, object-based) reference frame. Moreover, a stimulus that was displaced within a nonspatial reference frame did not induce rivalry despite the presence of spatial conflict. This finding shows that nonspatial, object-based processing can overrule retinotopic processing and prevent rivalry from occurring when a perceived stimulus exists unambiguously in an object-based reference frame. Our results identify a potent high-level conflict-resolution stage independent of low-level spatial visual conflict. This independence of spatial overlap provides an advantage to the visual system, allowing conflict resolution when an object is nonstationary on the retina (e.g., during frequently occurring eye movements). 相似文献
186.
Koch EF 《Psychological services》2012,9(2):203-205
The VA Maryland Health Care System introduced videoconferencing technology to provide psychiatry, evidenced-based psychotherapy, case management, and patient education at rural clinics where it was difficult to recruit providers. Telemental health services enable rural clinics to offer additional services, such as case management and patient education. Services have been expanded to urban outpatient clinics where a limited number of mental health clinic hours are available. This technology expands the availability of mental health providers and services, allowing patients to receive services from providers located at distant medical centers. 相似文献
187.
In order to interpret a constantly changing environment, visual events far apart in space and time must be integrated into a unified percept. While spatial properties of invisible signals are known to be encoded without awareness, the fate of temporal properties remains largely unknown. Here, we probed temporal integration for two distinct motion stimuli that were either visible or rendered invisible using continuous flash suppression. We found that when invisible, both the direction of apparent motion and the gender of point-light walkers were processed only when defined across short time periods (i.e., respectively 100 ms and 1000 ms). This limitation was not observed under full visibility. These similar findings at two different hierarchical levels of processing suggest that temporal integration windows shrink in the absence of perceptual awareness. We discuss this phenomenon as a key prediction of the global neuronal workspace and the information integration theories of consciousness. 相似文献
188.
Natalie Koch 《Sport, Ethics and Philosophy》2020,14(3):355-376
ABSTRACT The names of two major Gulf airlines, Qatar Airways and Emirates, have saturated the European football scene for many years, sponsoring some of the most prominent European teams and FIFA itself. These state-backed airlines are also active in motorsports, rugby, cycling, tennis, golf, cricket, and equestrian sport, while several prominent Gulf elites and royal family members have recently taken over major sports franchises in Europe and elsewhere. How should we understand these far-reaching sponsorship agendas in the Gulf? What can they tell us about the politics and ethics of international sport on the Arabian Peninsula? Moving beyond the general readings of Gulf sport sponsorship as an exercise in ‘soft power,’ this article shows how these deals are strategic nodes for diverse actors in the Gulf and in the international sporting community to advance various interests: personal, political, financial, and otherwise. Informed by a critical geopolitics lens that questions the coherence of the ‘state’ as an actor, I ask what it means to say that ‘the Gulf’ sponsors sport, and more specifically investigate the relevant actors behind these sponsorship deals. To do so, this article examines regional and global political economy through a focus on three Gulf airline sponsors, Emirates, Etihad, and Qatar Airways, and three elite sports sponsors—the UAE’s Sheik Mansour, Qatar’s Nasser bin Ghanim Al-Khelaïfi, and Sheikh Nasser of Bahrain. By decentering ‘soft power’ approaches to sport that unduly emphasize the ‘state’ as an actor, this article suggests a more grounded approach to the geopolitics of sport in the Arabian Peninsula, which simultaneously acknowledges the complicity of Western actors and institutions in the rise of Gulf sports sponsorship deals in the past decade. 相似文献
189.
190.
The present research demonstrates a so far unrecognized impediment of group performance, metacognitive myopia (Fiedler, 2012). Judges and decision-makers follow the given samples of information uncritically and neglect the metacognitive assessment of the samples' validity. Applying this notion to dyadic judgments, we instructed dyads to jointly estimate conditional probabilities p (Win|A) and p (Win|B) of Lotteries A and B. One person per dyad experienced a valid sample (winning rates conditional on lotteries). The other person experienced an invalid, reverse sample (lotteries conditional on winning). Whereas valid samples provide unbiased estimates of lotteries' winning probabilities, invalid samples can greatly misrepresent the association of winning and lotteries (depending on lottery base rates). Across three experiments, metacognitive myopia—both at the individual and at the dyadic group level—prevented participants from discriminating valid and invalid samples. Group judgments were biased toward erroneous implications of invalid samples, reflecting an equality bias among unequal group members. 相似文献