首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   188215篇
  免费   8365篇
  国内免费   155篇
  2020年   2783篇
  2019年   3445篇
  2018年   3665篇
  2017年   4110篇
  2016年   4706篇
  2015年   3930篇
  2014年   4804篇
  2013年   23452篇
  2012年   4981篇
  2011年   4296篇
  2010年   4101篇
  2009年   4859篇
  2008年   4423篇
  2007年   4053篇
  2006年   4495篇
  2005年   4397篇
  2004年   3883篇
  2003年   3479篇
  2002年   3331篇
  2001年   4048篇
  2000年   3917篇
  1999年   3661篇
  1998年   2892篇
  1997年   2729篇
  1996年   2643篇
  1995年   2451篇
  1994年   2412篇
  1993年   2390篇
  1992年   3041篇
  1991年   2845篇
  1990年   2690篇
  1989年   2550篇
  1988年   2537篇
  1987年   2526篇
  1986年   2565篇
  1985年   2754篇
  1984年   2753篇
  1983年   2504篇
  1982年   2448篇
  1981年   2417篇
  1980年   2248篇
  1979年   2529篇
  1978年   2328篇
  1977年   2245篇
  1976年   2054篇
  1975年   2239篇
  1974年   2317篇
  1973年   2252篇
  1972年   1836篇
  1971年   1728篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
401.
402.
403.
404.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
405.
Abstract

This study builds on and extends previous sales leadership research by exploring sales professionals’ perceptions of effective leadership behaviors. Semistructured interviews with both sales leaders and salespeople working in a global enterprise software company were examined through a qualitative analysis. Results indicated that participants believed sales leadership played an important role in influencing sales performance. When asked to describe specific sales leader behaviors that best enable salesperson performance, sales professionals – both sales leaders and salespeople – overwhelmingly referenced coaching, followed by collaborating, championing, and customer engaging. We define and describe these four key sales leader behaviors and identify four potential mediating variables (trust, confidence, optimism, and resilience), from which emerges a conceptual framework of sales leader behaviors perceived to enable salesperson performance. We examine these four key sales leader behaviors and mediators in the broader context of leadership theory, particularly transformational, servant, authentic, and adaptive leadership theories. The key contribution of this study is the identification of a set of leader behaviors that are likely to be especially effective in modern sales organizations given that they originated from the perceptions of sales professionals themselves.  相似文献   
406.
407.
There is an apparent tension in our everyday moral responsibility practices. On the one hand, it is commonly assumed that moral responsibility requires voluntary control: an agent can be morally responsible only for those things that fall within the scope of her voluntary control. On the other hand, we regularly praise and blame individuals for mental states and conditions that appear to fall outside the scope of their voluntary control, such as desires, emotions, beliefs, and other attitudes. In order to resolve this apparent tension, many philosophers appeal to a tracing principle to argue that agents are morally responsible (only) for those attitudes whose existence can be traced back, causally, to a voluntary action or omission in the past. My aim in this article is to critically evaluate this tracing strategy and to argue that it gives us a misguided picture of when and why we are morally responsible for our attitudes. I argue that we should accept a ‘judgment sensitivity’ condition of moral responsibility rather than a ‘voluntary control’ condition, and defend this account against various objections.  相似文献   
408.
409.
410.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号