全文获取类型
收费全文 | 195篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
196篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 26篇 |
2012年 | 15篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 3篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 1篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有196条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
122.
Yang Seok Cho Robert W. Proctor 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2005,58(5):839-864
Two types of stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) effect occur with orthogonal stimulus and response sets, an overall up-right/down-left advantage and mapping preferences that vary with response position. Researchers agree that the former type is due to asymmetric coding of the stimulus and response alternatives, but disagree as to whether the latter type requires a different explanation in terms of the properties of the motor system. This issue is examined in three experiments. The location of the stimulus set influenced orthogonal SRC when it varied along the same dimension as the responses (Experiments 1 and 2), with the pattern predicted by the hypothesis that the stimulus set provides a referent relative to which response position is coded. The effect of stimulus-set location on orthogonal SRC was independent of the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) for a marker that indicated stimulus-set side and the imperative stimulus. In contrast, a spatial correspondence effect for the irrelevant stimulus-set location and response was a decreasing function of SOA. Experiment 3 showed that the orthogonal SRC effect was determined by response position relative to the stimulus-set location and not the body midline. The results support the view that both types of orthogonal SRC effects are due to asymmetric coding of the stimuli and responses. 相似文献
123.
Hyoju Kim;Annie Tremblay;Taehong Cho; 《Cognitive Science》2024,48(12):e70026
This study investigates whether listeners’ cue weighting predicts their real-time use of asynchronous acoustic information in spoken word recognition at both group and individual levels. By focusing on the time course of cue integration, we seek to distinguish between two theoretical views: the associated view (cue weighting is linked to cue integration strategy) and the independent view (no such relationship). The current study examines Seoul Korean listeners’ (n = 62) weighting of voice onset time (VOT, available earlier in time) and onset fundamental frequency of the following vowel (F0, available later in time) when perceiving Korean stop contrasts (Experiment 1: cue-weighting perception task) and the timing of VOT integration when recognizing Korean words that begin with a stop (Experiment 2: visual-world eye-tracking task). The group-level results reveal that the timing of the early cue (VOT) integration is delayed when the later cue (F0) serves as the primary cue to process the stop contrast, supporting a relationship between cue weighting and the timing of cue integration (the associated view). At the individual level, listeners with greater reliance on F0 than VOT exhibited a further delayed integration of VOT. These findings suggest that the real-time processing of asynchronously occurring acoustic cues for lexical activation is modulated by the weight that listeners assign to those cues, providing evidence for the associated view of cue integration. This study offers insights into the mechanisms of cue integration and spoken word recognition, and they shed light on variability in cue integration strategies among listeners. 相似文献
124.
Seung Zeon Han Satoshi Semboshi Jee Hyuk Ahn Eun-Ae Choi Minah Cho Yusuke Kadoi 《Philosophical Magazine Letters》2013,93(8):275-283
ABSTRACTDeformation bands, especially shear bands were intentionally formed in supersaturated Cu-4 at.% Ti alloy by groove-rolling with 75% area reduction. Cold deformation prior to the ageing process changed the precipitation behaviour of the alloy. The deformed structure with shear bands was maintained, without showing recovery and recrystallisation, even after prolonged ageing at 450°C for more than one day. The thermally stable shear bands act as nucleation initiation sites and prevent the expansion of discontinuous cellular precipitates. The hardness reached a value of 305?Hv and an electrical conductivity of 15% IACS (IACS?=?International Annealed Copper Standard. 100% IACS is defined as the conductivity corresponding to a volume resistivity at 20°C of 17.241?nΩ), compared to 283?Hv and 13% IACS for conventional solid-solutioned and peak-hardened alloy. In addition, the ageing time to reach the highest hardness was shortened from 720 to 180?min at 450°C. 相似文献
125.
Russell E. Johnson Stanley B. Silverman Aarti Shyamsunder Hsien-Yao Swee O. Burcu Rodopman Eunae Cho 《人类行为》2013,26(5):403-427
Accounts of arrogant employees abound, yet there is little systematic research on arrogance within organizations. In response to this oversight, this article presents the findings from four studies. In Studies 1 and 2, the authors developed the Workplace Arrogance Scale and found support for its convergent and discriminant validity. In Study 3, the Workplace Arrogance Scale was included as part of a 360-degree performance feedback survey. Results revealed that there was satisfactory agreement between self- and other-ratings of arrogance. The authors also found that arrogance was negatively related to self- and other-rated task performance. Findings from Study 4 suggested that arrogance is negatively related to cognitive ability and self-esteem. The authors conclude by discussing the theoretical and practical implications of these findings. 相似文献
126.
A 13-item Individualism-Collectivism scale comprising source of identity, goal priority, mode of social relation, and norm acceptance is presented. A validation of this scale was conducted using a survey of 773 Korean employees. An exploratory factor analysis and a second-order confirmatory factor analysis supported the measure as having theoretical face validity and acceptable internal consistency reliability. Among the four facets, goal priority most strongly predicted the general Individualism-Collectivism latent factor. 相似文献
127.
McBride-Chang C Zhou Y Cho JR Aram D Levin I Tolchinsky L 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2011,109(2):256-262
Does learning to read influence one’s visual skill? In Study 1, kindergartners from Hong Kong, Korea, Israel, and Spain were tested on word reading and a task of visual spatial skill. Chinese and Korean kindergartners significantly outperformed Israeli and Spanish readers on the visual task. Moreover, in all cultures except Korea, good readers scored significantly higher on the visual task than did less good readers. In Study 2, we followed 215 Hong Kong Chinese kindergartners across 1 year, with word reading and visual skills tested twice. In this study, word reading at Time 1 by itself predicted 13% of unique variance in visual skill at Time 2. Together, these results underscore the potential importance of the process of learning to read for shaping one’s visual spatial skill development. 相似文献
128.
Young I. Cho Monica J. Martin Rand D. Conger Keith F. Widaman 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(2):157-168
We investigated measurement equivalence in two antisocial behavior scales (i.e., one scale for adolescents and a second scale
for young adults) by examining differential item functioning (DIF) for respondents from single-parent (n = 109) and two-parent families (n = 447). Even though one item in the scale for adolescents and two items in the scale for young adults showed significant
DIF, the two scales exhibited non-significant differential test functioning (DTF). Both uniform and nonuniform DIF were investigated
and examples of each type were identified. Specifically, uniform DIF was exhibited in the adolescent scale whereas nonuniform
DIF was shown in the young adult scale. Implications of DIF results for assessment of antisocial behavior, along with strengths
and limitations of the study, are discussed. 相似文献
129.
Baek-Hwan Cho Saebyul Kim Dong Ik Shin Jang Han Lee Sang Min Lee In Young Kim Sun I Kim 《Cyberpsychology & behavior》2004,7(5):519-526
In this research, the effectiveness of neurofeedback, along with virtual reality (VR), in reducing the level of inattention and impulsiveness was investigated. Twenty-eight male participants, aged 14-18, with social problems, took part in this study. They were separated into three groups: a control group, a VR group, and a non-VR group. The VR and non-VR groups underwent eight sessions of neurofeedback training over 2 weeks, while the control group just waited during the same period. The VR group used a head-mounted display (HMD) and a head tracker, which let them look around the virtual world. Conversely, the non-VR group used only a computer monitor with a fixed viewpoint. All participants performed a continuous performance task (CPT) before and after the complete training session. The results showed that both the VR and non-VR groups achieved better scores in the CPT after the training session, while the control group showed no significant difference. Compared with the other groups, the VR group presented a tendency to get better results, suggesting that immersive VR is applicable to neurofeedback for the rehabilitation of inattention and impulsiveness. 相似文献
130.