全文获取类型
收费全文 | 830篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
842篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 100篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 43篇 |
2010年 | 27篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 45篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 29篇 |
2003年 | 32篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1938年 | 4篇 |
1935年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有842条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
231.
We examined figural aftereffects in images of human faces, for which changes in configuration are highly discriminable. Observers
either matched or rated faces before or after viewing distorted images of faces. Prior adaptation strongly biases face perception
by causing the original face to appear distorted in a direction opposite to the adapting distortion. Aftereffects transferred
across different faces and were similar for upright or inverted faces, but were weaker when the adapting and test faces had
different orientations (e.g., adapt inverted and test upright). Thus the aftereffects depend on which images are distorted,
and not simply on the type of distortion introduced. We further show that the aftereffects are asymmetric, for adapting to
the original face has little effect on the perception of a distorted face. This asymmetry suggests that adaptation may play
an important normalizing role in face perception. Our results suggest that in normal viewing, figural aftereffects may strongly
influence form perception and could provide a novel method for probing properties of human face perception. 相似文献
232.
Ziadni Maisa S. You Dokyoung S. Sturgeon John A. Mackey Sean C. Darnall Beth D. 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2021,28(2):349-360
Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings - Cumulative evidence supports the association between perceived childhood neglect and adulthood psychological and physical health. To date,... 相似文献
233.
McCrea SM 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2008,95(2):274-292
Researchers interested in counterfactual thinking have often found that upward counterfactual thoughts lead to increased motivation to improve in the future, although at the cost of increased negative affect. The present studies suggest that because upward counterfactual thoughts indicate reasons for a poor performance, they can also serve as excuses. In this case, upward counterfactual thoughts should result in more positive self-esteem and reduced future motivation. Five studies demonstrated these effects in the context of self-handicapping. First, upward counterfactual thinking was increased in the presence of a self-handicap. Second, upward counterfactual thoughts indicating the presence of a self-handicap protected self-esteem following failure. Finally, upward counterfactual thoughts that protect self-esteem reduced preparation for a subsequent performance as well as performance itself. These findings suggest that the consequences of upward counterfactuals for affect and motivation are moderated by the goals of the individual as well as the content of the thoughts. 相似文献
234.
Early dialogues between parent and child constitute an important factor for the acquisition of culture and hence verbal interaction is considered to be a universal parenting system. Parenting strategies and socialization practices are strongly influenced by the cultural conception of the self, prototypically defined as the model of independence and interdependence. Our study focuses on the temporal organization of spontaneous verbal/vocal behavior of 20 German middle-class and 28 Cameroonian Nso mother–infant dyads. The infants and their mothers were observed weekly in a 5 min free-play interaction scene from 0 to 3 months of age. We hypothesized to find different amounts of vocalization time, synchronous vocalizations, and contingent maternal responses in the verbal/vocal patterns of the two samples. The findings indicate cross-cultural differences in the temporal structure of verbal/vocal interactions already during the first three months of life, reflecting underlying differences in the culture-specific modes of verbal interaction. 相似文献
235.
Dennis M. Kivlighan Joseph A. Johnston R. Sean Hogan Ellen Mauer 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1994,72(3):289-292
The Adaptive Counseling and Therapy (ACT: Howard, Nance, & Myers, 1986) model was used to predict who would benefit from computerized career counseling. Specifically, clients who had more stable goals and aspirations and more defensive independence were expected to derive more benefit from computerized career counseling, an intervention that provides low levels of direction and support. A total of 54 clients who came to the Career Planning and Placement Center seeking career assistance were referred to the System for Interactive Guidance and Information PLUS (SIGI-PLUS). As hypothesized, clients with more stable goals had the greatest gains in vocational identity. Additionally, clients who showed more defensive independence were more satisfied with the SIGI-PLUS treatment. Implications, limitations, and suggestions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
236.
Benjamin Otto Supriya Misra Aditya Prasad Kateri McRae 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2014,14(3):923-938
One factor that influences the success of emotion regulation is the manner in which the regulated emotion was generated. Recent research has suggested that reappraisal, a top-down emotion regulation strategy, is more effective in decreasing self-reported negative affect when emotions were generated from the top-down, versus the bottom-up. On the basis of a process overlap framework, we hypothesized that the neural regions active during reappraisal would overlap more with emotions that were generated from the top-down, rather than from the bottom-up. In addition, we hypothesized that increased neural overlap between reappraisal and the history effects of top-down emotion generation would be associated with increased reappraisal success. The results of several analyses suggested that reappraisal and emotions that were generated from the top-down share a core network of prefrontal, temporal, and cingulate regions. This overlap is specific; no such overlap was observed between reappraisal and emotions that were generated in a bottom-up fashion. This network consists of regions previously implicated in linguistic processing, cognitive control, and self-relevant appraisals, which are processes thought to be crucial to both reappraisal and top-down emotion generation. Furthermore, individuals with high reappraisal success demonstrated greater neural overlap between reappraisal and the history of top-down emotion generation than did those with low reappraisal success. The overlap of these key regions, reflecting overlapping processes, provides an initial insight into the mechanism by which generation history may facilitate emotion regulation. 相似文献
237.
Development of achievement-related motives in young athletes is believed to be influenced by the motivational climate created
by coaches. In a longitudinal multilevel design utilizing 47 youth basketball teams, coach-initiated motivational climate
was used to predict changes in 9–13 year old athletes’ achievement goal orientations over the course of a season. Mastery
climate scores on the Motivational Climate Scale for Youth Sports were associated with significant increases in mastery goal
orientation and decreases in ego orientation scores on the Achievement Goal Scale for Youth Sports. Ego motivational climate
scores were significantly related to increases in ego goal orientation scores. These relations were not influenced by athletes’
age or gender. Intraclass correlations indicated low within-team consensus in athletes’ motivational climate scores, suggesting
an individual- rather than team-level perceptual construct. These and other findings indicate that achievement goal orientation
research can be extended downward to children below the age of 11.
相似文献
Ronald E. SmithEmail: |
238.
The extent to which readers can exert strategic control over oral reading processes is a matter of debate. According to the
pathway control hypothesis, the relative contributions of the lexical and nonlexical pathways can be modulated by the characteristics
of the context stimuli being read, but an alternative time criterion model is also a viable explanation of past results. In
Experiment 1, subjects named high- and low-frequency regular words in the context of either low-frequency exception words
(e.g., pint) or nonwords (e.g., flirp). Frequency effects (faster pronunciation latencies for high-frequency words) were attenuated in the nonword context, consistent
with the notion that nonwords emphasize the characteristics of the frequency-insensitive nonlexical pathway. Importantly,
we also assessed memory for targets, and a similar attenuation of the frequency effect in recognition memory was observed
in the nonword condition. Converging evidence was obtained in a second experiment in which a variable that was more sensitive
to the nonlexical pathway (orthographic neighborhood size) was manipulated. The results indicated that both speeded pronunciation
performance and memory performance were relatively attenuated in the low-frequency exception word context in comparision with
the nonword context. The opposing influences of list context type for word frequency and orthographic neighborhood size effects
in speeded pronunciation and memory performance provide strong support for the pathway control model, as opposed to the time
criterion model. 相似文献
239.
Sean T. Ma James L. Abelson Go Okada Stephan F. Taylor Israel Liberzon 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2017,17(2):437-451
Psychosocial well-being requires effective regulation of emotional responding in context of threat or stress. Neuroimaging studies have focused on instructed, volitional regulation (e.g., reappraisal or distancing), largely ignoring implicit regulation that does not involve purposeful effort to alter emotional experience. These implicit processes may or may not involve the same neural pathways as explicit regulatory strategies. We examined the neurobiology of implicit emotional regulation processes and the impact of the stress hormone cortisol on these processes. Our study task employed composite pictures of faces and places to examine neural activity during implicit emotional processing (of emotional faces), while these responses were implicitly regulated by attention shift away from the emotionally evocative stimuli, and while subjects reflectively appraised their own emotional response to them. Subjects completed the task in an fMRI scanner after random assignment to receive placebo or hydrocortisone (HCT), an orally administered version of cortisol. Implicit emotional processing activated insula/IFG, dACC/dMPFC, midbrain and amygdala. With attention shifting, we saw diminished signal in emotion generating/response regions (e.g., amygdala) and increased activations in task specific attention regions like parahippocampus. With appraisal of emotions, we observed robust activations in medial prefrontal areas, where activation is also seen in instructed reappraisal studies. We observed no main effects of HCT administration on brain, but males and females showed opposing neural effects in prefrontal areas. The data suggest that different types of emotion regulation utilize overlapping circuits, but with some strategy specific activation. Further study of the dimorphic sex response to cortisol is needed. 相似文献
240.