全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7095篇 |
免费 | 293篇 |
专业分类
7388篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 100篇 |
2020年 | 149篇 |
2019年 | 462篇 |
2018年 | 330篇 |
2017年 | 452篇 |
2016年 | 451篇 |
2015年 | 414篇 |
2014年 | 405篇 |
2013年 | 814篇 |
2012年 | 405篇 |
2011年 | 340篇 |
2010年 | 309篇 |
2009年 | 300篇 |
2008年 | 318篇 |
2007年 | 256篇 |
2006年 | 240篇 |
2005年 | 206篇 |
2004年 | 175篇 |
2003年 | 167篇 |
2002年 | 195篇 |
2001年 | 125篇 |
2000年 | 106篇 |
1999年 | 64篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 49篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 23篇 |
1992年 | 34篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有7388条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
972.
In this exploratory qualitative study, 15 mothers and 18 adolescents who, as family members of the South Lebanon Army, were uprooted from that country and now living in Israel, were interviewed separately. The mothers described their image of the adaptive adult, which stressed the positive values of loyalty to the family, orientation toward the group, respect, and acceptance of hierarchy and guided their child‐rearing goals and socialization. The children shared similar attitudes about the importance of family loyalty and orientation toward the group, but differed from their mothers with regard to the issues of respect and obedience. We discussed the notion of “liminality,” to further our understanding of the families in transition and suggest how it applies to the uprooted members of South Lebanon army. We suggest that different time orientations and contextual factors influence the participating mothers’ image of the adaptive adult and the degree to which their children accept it. Implications for research and interventions are also explored. 相似文献
973.
974.
975.
Magdalena A. Żemojtel‐Piotrowska Jarosław P. Piotrowski Amanda Clinton 《International journal of psychology》2016,51(3):196-204
In this study, the relationship between agency, communion, and the active, passive, and revenge forms of entitlement is examined. Results indicate that active entitlement was positively related to agency, negatively to communion (Study 1), and unrelated to unmitigated agency and communion (Study 2). Passive entitlement was positively related to communion (in regular and unmitigated forms) and negatively related to agency (in both forms). Revenge entitlement was positively related to agency (unmitigated and regular), and negatively related to both regular and unmitigated communal orientations. Detected relationships were independent from self‐esteem (Study 1). The findings are discussed in relation to distinctions between narcissistic and healthy entitlement, and within the context of the three‐dimensional model of entitlement. 相似文献
976.
Pei Wang Chen‐Hao Tan Xiang‐Xia Zhao Yong‐He Liu Chong‐De Lin 《International journal of psychology》2016,51(2):156-162
In this study, a priming Stroop paradigm was used to determine whether stereotype activation is unintentional. Priming conditions (priming/no‐priming) and the relationship between priming and target (consistent/inconsistent/no‐relation) were the independent variables; accuracy, reaction time and N400 amplitude were used as dependent variables. The reaction time revealed that stereotype activation is, to some extent, unintentional. Furthermore, the event‐related potenial (ERP) results showed that N400 amplitude was larger for inconsistent conditions than for consistent conditions. This result supported the notion that stereotype activation is an unintentional and automatic process. 相似文献
977.
Jennifer E. Lansford Jennifer Godwin Liane Peña Alampay Liliana Maria Uribe Tirado Arnaldo Zelli Suha M. Al‐Hassan Dario Bacchini Anna Silvia Bombi Marc H. Bornstein Lei Chang Kirby Deater‐Deckard Laura Di Giunta Kenneth A. Dodge Patrick S. Malone Paul Oburu Concetta Pastorelli Ann T. Skinner Emma Sorbring Sombat Tapanya 《International journal of psychology》2016,51(5):366-374
Children's family obligations involve assistance and respect that children are expected to provide to immediate and extended family members and reflect beliefs related to family life that may differ across cultural groups. Mothers, fathers and children (N = 1432 families) in 13 cultural groups in 9 countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Sweden, Thailand and United States) reported on their expectations regarding children's family obligations and parenting attitudes and behaviours. Within families, mothers and fathers had more concordant expectations regarding children's family obligations than did parents and children. Parenting behaviours that were warmer, less neglectful and more controlling as well as parenting attitudes that were more authoritarian were related to higher expectations regarding children's family obligations between families within cultures as well as between cultures. These international findings advance understanding of children's family obligations by contextualising them both within families and across a number of diverse cultural groups in 9 countries. 相似文献
978.
James R. Lewis Sean E. Currie Michael P. Oman‐Reagan 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2016,55(1):91-104
A number of different studies carried out in the late 20th century indicated that new religious movements (NRMs) tended to recruit individuals who were highly educated. In the present study, we confirm this pattern utilizing data from the national censuses of Australia, New Zealand, Canada, England, and Wales. Additionally, we found that educational patterns for NRMs in the censuses tended to fall into at least two subgroups, one of which had educational levels comparable to mainline denominations and the other of which had significantly higher educational achievements. Furthermore, census respondents who expressed some variety of nonbelief were comparable to this latter group in terms of educational accomplishments. We discuss this latter finding in terms of Ernst Troeltsch and Colin Campbell's analysis of secularization. 相似文献
979.
Jun‐Ichiro Kawahara 《The Japanese psychological research》2016,58(3):261-272
Currently, some Japanese women use a sanitary mask to hide their faces when not wearing makeup, perhaps because they believe that they are more attractive (or less ugly) when wearing a sanitary mask than when not wearing makeup. The present study examined the effect of wearing a sanitary mask on the perception of facial attractiveness. We manipulated the presence or absence of a mask in the main experiments or an occluder (e.g., notebook) in control experiments and asked participants to rate facial images. The results revealed that attractive faces wearing a sanitary mask were perceived as less attractive than the same faces without the mask, contrary to Japanese women's belief. This is the first study to demonstrate a new phenomenon, the sanitary‐mask effect, in which observers underestimate the physical attractiveness of a mask‐wearing face. Importantly, the pattern of the results of perceived attractiveness was substantially altered when a control object occluded the faces. This suggests that facial occlusion by a sanitary mask has a unique effect, due to occlusion and unhealthiness priming. 相似文献
980.
Jan De Houwer Yvonne Barnes‐Holmes Dermot Barnes‐Holmes 《International journal of psychology》2016,51(1):40-44
Different types of therapy explain psychopathology and the effects of psychotherapy differently. Different explanations are, however, not necessarily mutually exclusive. Based on the idea that functional and cognitive explanations are situated at different levels, we argue that functional therapies such as traditional Behaviour Therapy (BT) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) are not necessarily incompatible with Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT). Whether a functional and a cognitive therapy actually align depends on whether they highlight the same type of environmental causes. This functional‐cognitive perspective reveals various differences and communalities among BT, CBT and ACT. 相似文献