首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   680篇
  免费   42篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   15篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   14篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   70篇
  2012年   22篇
  2011年   27篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   21篇
  2007年   19篇
  2006年   21篇
  2005年   22篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   22篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   26篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   13篇
  1996年   6篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   6篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   13篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   15篇
  1982年   9篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   13篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   11篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   9篇
  1973年   9篇
  1972年   8篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   6篇
  1968年   7篇
  1967年   10篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有722条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
Although emotion research and clinical practice often implicitly entail judgments about the patterns and situational appropriateness of a person's emotional reactions, we have little empirical knowledge of how emotions typically interrelate during various affective situations. To assess patterns of response to normative situations, a questionnaire was constructed and validated to include items primarily eliciting one of six emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, depression, and anxiety) and one of three intensities (low, moderate, and high). A total of 216 persons rated one of four forms of the questionnaire on each of the six emotional categories. Each of the six types of situations generated significantly different patterns of emotion. As Izard (1972) predicted, depression situations elicited more complex patterns of emotion than sadness situations, including significantly more anger, fear, and anxiety. In contrast, anxiety situations did not elicit more complex patterns than fear situations. Rather, the evidence was more consistent with the conclusion that fear is a particular type of anxiety. Various other relations among specific emotions are discussed, as well as some subtle sex differences and intensity effects. The data highlight the sensitivity with which the assessment of patterns of emotions associated with specific situations can produce new information about the nature of affective experience.  相似文献   
162.
163.
164.
Drawing on concepts and data from psychoanalysis, neuroscience, ethology, and experimental psychology, this paper suggests that affects are the prime movers of human motivation and have, in addition, these properties: they are neurophysiologically generated, sensationlike signals originating not in classical sensory systems, but in limbic structures; activation of the neurobiologic affect generators reliably triggers motor stereotype--like facial expression, posture, and tone of voice--which in turn transmit the physical data underlying empathy and emotional communication; and appetitively sought pleasant emotions-sensations and aversive dysphoric feelings serve respectively as the ultimate, "brain-synthesized" positive and negative reinforcers of emotional learning. The latter ideas point to derivative hypotheses: that parental nonverbal behavior and communication evoke in children the affects that guide character development and choice of defense, and that these processes are consistent with an affect-reinforced associative learning model comprising both operant and classical conditioning paradigms. Psychoanalysis as therapy, then, may mechanistically rely for its clinical effectiveness on extinctionlike processes in which new experience in the analyst-patient relationship leads to the modulation or extinction of initially crippling signal affects associatively linked to various other affects, appetites, and behaviors. Finally, the core hypotheses mesh both with recent findings from the cellular study of affect and learning mechanisms and with associative learning-unlearning assumptions variably explicit in psychoanalytic writings since the time of Freud's (1895) Project.  相似文献   
165.
166.
167.
168.
169.
170.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号