首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   69536篇
  免费   2645篇
  国内免费   23篇
  72204篇
  2019年   742篇
  2018年   1062篇
  2017年   1024篇
  2016年   1164篇
  2015年   858篇
  2014年   1008篇
  2013年   5737篇
  2012年   1849篇
  2011年   1852篇
  2010年   1187篇
  2009年   1129篇
  2008年   1721篇
  2007年   1618篇
  2006年   1455篇
  2005年   1318篇
  2004年   1251篇
  2003年   1210篇
  2002年   1197篇
  2001年   2185篇
  2000年   2114篇
  1999年   1577篇
  1998年   730篇
  1996年   771篇
  1995年   721篇
  1994年   727篇
  1992年   1584篇
  1991年   1449篇
  1990年   1451篇
  1989年   1283篇
  1988年   1268篇
  1987年   1253篇
  1986年   1275篇
  1985年   1314篇
  1984年   1091篇
  1983年   969篇
  1982年   709篇
  1981年   718篇
  1979年   1135篇
  1978年   787篇
  1976年   725篇
  1975年   996篇
  1974年   1078篇
  1973年   1180篇
  1972年   1063篇
  1971年   970篇
  1970年   915篇
  1969年   892篇
  1968年   1142篇
  1967年   1016篇
  1966年   924篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 18 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
44.
45.
46.
47.
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers.  相似文献   
48.
49.
50.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号