全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1078篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
1105篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 34篇 |
2006年 | 38篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 30篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 17篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 10篇 |
1969年 | 14篇 |
1968年 | 19篇 |
1967年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1105条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Witte TK Fitzpatrick KK Warren KL Schatschneider C Schmidt NB 《Behaviour research and therapy》2006,44(7):1029-1040
Suicidal ideation has been thought to have a relatively stable course across weeks and months. However, daily changes in levels of ideation have not been adequately examined despite the importance of potential variability clinically and conceptually. For example, it has been suggested that variability in suicidal ideation may become less closely tied to variability in other mood symptoms (e.g., depression, hopelessness) among individuals with multiple suicide attempts. The present report had two related goals: (1) to prospectively evaluate suicidal ideation and related mood symptoms, and (2) to determine whether suicide attempt status predicted a decreased association between ideation and other mood symptoms. Non-clinical participants (N=108) with varying levels of suicidal ideation and number of previous suicide attempts completed the beck hopelessness scale (BHS), beck depression inventory (BDI), and suicide probability scale (SPS) every day for 4 weeks. Findings suggested considerable variability in suicidal ideation, especially for multiple attempters. Multiple attempt status predicted a decreased association between suicidal ideation and depression, although the results were only marginally significant. These findings have implications for conceptual models of suicide risk as well as assessment and treatment of suicidal individuals. 相似文献
72.
Norman Doe 《International Journal for the Study of the Christian Church》2013,13(2):93-111
An important recent development in worldwide Anglicanism is the emergence over recent years of a project to articulate the principles of canon law common to the churches of the Anglican Communion. This project seeks to express the juridical character of Anglicanism from a global perspective, not only to underscore the many fundamental values that Anglicans share in terms of their polity, ministry, doctrine, liturgy, rites and property, going to the very roots of Anglican identity but, also, as a concrete resource for other churches in ecumenical dialogue with Anglicans. This article traces the development of the so-called ius commune project, describes the methodological challenges which it faces and, the process of producing a draft. It also seeks to compare the project with the juridical experiences of other international ecclesial communities and, briefly, to place the project in the context of the debate about the adoption of an Anglican Covenant, an initiative proposed by the Lambeth Commission in 2004. 相似文献
73.
74.
Bias in Eyewitness Accounts: The Effects of Question Format,Delay Interval,and Stimulus Presentation
Thomas J. Lipscomb Hunter A. McAllister Norman J. Bregman 《The Journal of psychology》2013,147(3):207-212
One of three representations of a staged automobile collision was shown to 180 students from introductory psychology classes. We then questioned the students about details of the accident, using either marked or unmarked modifiers. Half the students were questioned immediately after viewing the stimulus material and half after a 20-min delay. The results indicated that estimates of the magnitude of a number of aspects of the collision were significantly greater when unmarked modifiers were used in phrasing the relevant questions. Students who were questioned after the 20-min delay gave significantly greater estimates of monetary damage than the students who answered immediately after viewing the representation. The nature of the stimulus material had inconsistent but significant effects. 相似文献
75.
76.
We address some concerns related to the use of post-trial attribution judgments, originally developed for artificial grammar learning (AGL), during the version of the serial reaction time (SRT) task used by Fu, Dienes, and Fu (2010). In particular, intuition attributions, which are central to Fu et al.’s arguments, seem problematic: This attribution is likely to be made when stimuli contain several competing sources of information to which subjective feelings could be attributed. The interpretation of intuition attributions in Fu et al.’s SRT generation task is problematic because the procedure involved a 2-element sequence where items varied only in position. In our view, responses categorised as intuitions might have been a variety of guess response where neither judgement knowledge nor structural knowledge were conscious. The results would then be compatible with previous findings showing that people can control the use of unconscious structural knowledge even when judgement knowledge is unconscious. 相似文献
77.
In response to concerns with existing procedures for measuring strategic control over implicit knowledge in artificial grammar learning (AGL), we introduce a more stringent measurement procedure. After two separate training blocks which each consisted of letter strings derived from a different grammar, participants either judged the grammaticality of novel letter strings with respect to only one of these two grammars (pure-block condition), or had the target grammar varying randomly from trial to trial (novel mixed-block condition) which required a higher degree of conscious flexible control. Random variation in the colour and font of letters was introduced to disguise the nature of the rule and reduce explicit learning. Strategic control was observed both in the pure-block and mixed-block conditions, and even among participants who did not realise the rule was based on letter identity. This indicated detailed strategic control in the absence of explicit learning. 相似文献
78.
The ability of observers to perceive three-dimensional (3-D) distances or lengths along intrinsically curved surfaces was investigated in three experiments. Three physically curved surfaces were used: convex and/or concave hemispheres (Experiments 1 and 3) and a hyperbolic paraboloid (Experiment 2). The first two experiments employed a visual length-matching task, but in the final experiment the observers estimated the surface lengths motorically by varying the separation between their two index fingers. In general, the observers' judgments of surface length in both tasks (perceptual vs. motoric matching) were very precise but were not necessarily accurate. Large individual differences (overestimation, underestimation, etc.) in the perception of length occurred. There were also significant effects of viewing distance, type of surface, and orientation of the spatial intervals on the observers' judgments of surface length. The individual differences and failures of perceptual constancy that were obtained indicate that there is no single relationship between physical and perceived distances on 3-D surfaces that is consistent across observers. 相似文献
79.
80.
Victor J. Perotti James T. Todd J. Farley Norman 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1996,58(5):666-679
Four experiments investigated observers’ judgments of rigidity for different types of optical motion. The depicted structural deformations were of two types: (1) those with nonparallel image trajectories that are detectable from the first-order spatiotemporal relations between pairs of views; and (2) those with parallel image trajectories that can only be detected from higher order relations among three or more views. Patterns were composed of smooth flow fields in Experiments 1 and 3, and of wire frame figures in Experiments 2 and 4. For both types of display, the nonrigidity detectable from the first-order spatiotemporal structure of the motion sequence was much more salient than the deformation detectable only from the higher order spatiotemporal structure. These results indicate that observers’ judgments of rigidity are based primarily on a two-view analysis, but that some useful information can be obtained under appropriate circumstances from higher order spatiotemporal relations among three or more views. 相似文献