全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8549篇 |
免费 | 367篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
8919篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 82篇 |
2021年 | 105篇 |
2020年 | 148篇 |
2019年 | 181篇 |
2018年 | 282篇 |
2017年 | 259篇 |
2016年 | 225篇 |
2015年 | 178篇 |
2014年 | 187篇 |
2013年 | 744篇 |
2012年 | 358篇 |
2011年 | 338篇 |
2010年 | 173篇 |
2009年 | 202篇 |
2008年 | 264篇 |
2007年 | 283篇 |
2006年 | 228篇 |
2005年 | 202篇 |
2004年 | 199篇 |
2003年 | 152篇 |
2002年 | 170篇 |
2001年 | 237篇 |
2000年 | 218篇 |
1999年 | 163篇 |
1998年 | 76篇 |
1996年 | 69篇 |
1993年 | 66篇 |
1992年 | 126篇 |
1991年 | 129篇 |
1990年 | 127篇 |
1989年 | 107篇 |
1988年 | 94篇 |
1987年 | 103篇 |
1986年 | 117篇 |
1985年 | 110篇 |
1984年 | 100篇 |
1983年 | 87篇 |
1979年 | 98篇 |
1978年 | 72篇 |
1977年 | 64篇 |
1975年 | 67篇 |
1974年 | 94篇 |
1973年 | 100篇 |
1972年 | 74篇 |
1971年 | 69篇 |
1970年 | 80篇 |
1969年 | 75篇 |
1968年 | 106篇 |
1967年 | 82篇 |
1966年 | 93篇 |
排序方式: 共有8919条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
82.
ALLEN I. KRAUT 《Personnel Psychology》1969,22(3):281-290
T his study explores the relationship between high level managers' promotional success and two measures of intellectual ability. The Concept Mastery Test was administered to 235 middle managers and 130 higher level executives who attended advanced management training programs. The Ship Destination Test was also given to about half the trainees. Both tests are considered to discriminate well among superior individuals. Those managers who earned high scores were rated more favorably by their peers and the training staff. However, advancement in the 4 to 7 years after the training program was unrelated to test performance. 相似文献
83.
Judith I. Laszlo 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1967,19(4):344-349
Five subjects were trained to tap on a light Morse-key during nerve compression block. The training sessions lasted for 40 sec., with a 5 sec. rest after the first 20 sec. work period. The group learning curve reached 89.5 per cent. level of normal performance by the eighth training session. In the ninth, the testing session, subjects tapped with visual and auditory sense reduction superimposed on the kinaesthetic and tactile impariment of the training condition. Performance in the testing session reached 40.9 per cent. of normal.
The sixth subject was trained in the same task as the other five subjects, but the training condition included elimination of cues from all four sensory channels. He reached 79.09 per cent. of his normal tapping performance in the seventh session.
These results show that the motor skill of tapping can be relearned in the absence of kinaesthetic cues. Furthermore when the subject has no conscious knowledge of any peripheral sensory cues connected with the ongoing motor activity, learning can nevertheless take place. These findings lead to the hypothesis, that skilled motor activity can be monitored by central processes alone.
During the training sessions subjects showed a tendency of tapping in groups of gradually increasing length. It is hypothesized that increased number of taps forming a group gives an indication to the possible mode of action of these central processes. 相似文献
The sixth subject was trained in the same task as the other five subjects, but the training condition included elimination of cues from all four sensory channels. He reached 79.09 per cent. of his normal tapping performance in the seventh session.
These results show that the motor skill of tapping can be relearned in the absence of kinaesthetic cues. Furthermore when the subject has no conscious knowledge of any peripheral sensory cues connected with the ongoing motor activity, learning can nevertheless take place. These findings lead to the hypothesis, that skilled motor activity can be monitored by central processes alone.
During the training sessions subjects showed a tendency of tapping in groups of gradually increasing length. It is hypothesized that increased number of taps forming a group gives an indication to the possible mode of action of these central processes. 相似文献
84.
Two recent experiments (Moray, 1959; Oswald, Taylor and Treisman, 1960) have shown that in dichotic listening, and in sleep one's own name seems to be responded to selectively. We have shown for 10 subjects, each listening binaurally to the same recording of a list of their names repeated at random 10 times, and masked by noise, that the subject's own name had a significantly lower threshold than other names. The effect we found was not significantly different from the one found by Oswald et al. during sleep and by Moray for the dichotic situation.
These three experiments taken together suggest that the same pattern-analysing mechanism is involved in normal listening, dichotic listening to the rejected message, and in discrimination during sleep. 相似文献
These three experiments taken together suggest that the same pattern-analysing mechanism is involved in normal listening, dichotic listening to the rejected message, and in discrimination during sleep. 相似文献
85.
86.
87.
88.
89.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(5):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献
90.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(12):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献