首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7799篇
  免费   395篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2024年   18篇
  2023年   99篇
  2022年   131篇
  2021年   171篇
  2020年   226篇
  2019年   256篇
  2018年   387篇
  2017年   372篇
  2016年   326篇
  2015年   224篇
  2014年   260篇
  2013年   998篇
  2012年   561篇
  2011年   546篇
  2010年   374篇
  2009年   238篇
  2008年   467篇
  2007年   370篇
  2006年   342篇
  2005年   296篇
  2004年   195篇
  2003年   197篇
  2002年   183篇
  2001年   78篇
  2000年   105篇
  1999年   68篇
  1998年   39篇
  1997年   38篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   31篇
  1994年   28篇
  1993年   26篇
  1992年   41篇
  1991年   30篇
  1990年   32篇
  1989年   33篇
  1988年   30篇
  1987年   23篇
  1986年   16篇
  1985年   22篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   19篇
  1982年   15篇
  1981年   14篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   14篇
  1976年   15篇
  1974年   14篇
  1972年   15篇
  1965年   13篇
排序方式: 共有8199条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
Contributions of learning to human development   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
  相似文献   
993.
Heart rate (HR), operant key pressing, and urinary 11-hydroxycorticosteroids (11-OH-CS) were recorded while dogs were subjected to various aversive conditioning schedules over a six-month period. The schedules consisted of Sidman avoidance, followed by a Sidman schedule which paired unavoidable shocks (US) with offset of seven discrete conditioned stimuli (CS). Introduction of the Sidman avoidance schedule led to an increase in HR. Changes in the experimental schedule always results in an increase in urinary 11-OH-CS. Neither HR nor urinary 11-OH-CS were correlated with rate of operant key pressing. Differences in the dependent variables reflected the different topological characteristics of the dogs studied. This study demonstrated: (a) a causal independence of physiological and behavioral responses conditioned to the same stimulus complex, and (b) that longterm increases in HR can be maintained if the experimental situation is manipulated to maintain the fear eliciting characteristics of the conditioning situation.  相似文献   
994.
995.
The article explores Freud's writing on homosexuality, from his early hypotheses, expressed in his letters to Fleiss to his last observations in The Outline of Psychoanalysis, published in 1940 after his death. We trace the continuities as well as changes in his thinking, and have organized the paper conceptually, under the headings: 1) Bisexuality 2) Narcissism and Object choice, 3) On Normality and Pathology, and 4) The Quantitative factor and Aggression. We show that Freud was the first to confirm the existence of homosexualities, that he offers no black and white solution to the question of normality and pathology, although he contributes to the understanding of the vehemence that surrounds the subject, and that, in the considerable body of work, he has offered a rich and varied foundation for further thinking on the subject.  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
In Category Mistakes, Magidor proposes that sortal restrictions should be viewed as pragmatic presuppositions. This contrasts with recent linguistic theories of successfully resolved category mistakes, e.g. coercions or copredication. It has been argued that for the proper treatment of such examples, sortal restrictions should be expressed by semantic presuppositions since they need to interact with compositional semantics. I explore possible ways in which Magidor’s theory could be extended to explain examples of coercion and copredication. The outcome of the discussion is that incorporation of these facts into Magidor’s pragmatic system might be possible, but ecessitates choices that are far from trivial.  相似文献   
999.
1000.
The authors investigated the development of the affective, cognitive, and behavioral components of empathy in preschoolers, specifically examining how cognitive empathy is linked to theory of mind and affective perspective taking. Participants were 158 children aged 4–6 years. They listened to narratives and then answered questions about the protagonists' emotions. The affective component was probed with the question, “How do you feel seeing the little girl/boy?”; the cognitive component with the question, “Why do you feel [emotion shared with the character]?”; and the behavioral one with the question, “What would you do if you were next to the little boy/girl [experiencing an emotional scenario]?” Results revealed a developmental sequence in the self-focused attribution of cognitive empathy, and a trend toward a developmental sequence for behavioral empathy, which underwent a slight linear increase between 4 and 6 years old. Affective empathy remained stable. More interestingly, they showed that cognitive empathy is linked to both theory of mind and affective perspective taking.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号