排序方式: 共有42条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
A key criticism of the main diagnostic tool in psychiatry, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (DSM-IV), is that it lacks a biological footing. In this article, we argue for a biological approach to psychiatry based on 'neurocognitive endophenotypes', whereby changes in behavioural or cognitive processes are associated with discrete deficits in defined neural systems. We focus on the constructs of impulsivity and compulsivity as key examples of the approach and discuss their possible cross-diagnostic significance, applying them to co-morbidities and commonalities across a range of disorders (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, substance dependence, obsessive-compulsive disorder and eating disorders). We argue that this approach has important implications for the future classification of psychiatric disorders, genetics and therapeutics. 相似文献
32.
van der Ven SH Boom J Kroesbergen EH Leseman PP 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2012,113(1):1-19
Variability in strategy selection is an important characteristic of learning new skills such as mathematical skills. Strategies gradually come and go during this development. In 1996, Siegler described this phenomenon as "overlapping waves." In the current microgenetic study, we attempted to model these overlapping waves statistically. In addition, we investigated whether development in strategy selection is related to development in accuracy and to what degree working memory is related to both. We expected that children with poor working memory are limited in their possibilities to make the associations that are necessary to progress to more mature strategies. This limitation would explain the often-found relationship between working memory and mathematical abilities. To this aim, the strategy selection and accuracy of 98 children who were learning single-digit multiplication was assessed eight times on a weekly basis. Using latent growth modeling for categorical data, we confirmed Siegler's hypothesis of overlapping waves. Moreover, both the intercepts and the slopes of strategy selection and accuracy were strongly interrelated. Finally, working memory predicted both strategy selection and accuracy, confirming that working memory is related to mathematical problem solving in two ways because it influences both the maturity of strategy choice and the probability of making procedural mistakes. 相似文献
33.
34.
Erik W. de Kwaadsteniet Sanne A.M. Rijkhoff Eric van Dijk 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2013,120(2):251-259
The present paper focuses on third-parties’ decisions to punish and reward in social dilemmas, and on the moderating role of environmental uncertainty (i.e., uncertainty about the size of the common resource). We argue and demonstrate that in social dilemmas third-parties use the equality rule as a strict benchmark to determine punishments (Study 1) as well as rewards (Study 2), but only under environmental certainty. Under environmental uncertainty, third-parties do not apply such a strict benchmark to distinguish cooperators from defectors. Instead, they appear to use the following rule: the more an individual group member has cooperated the less he/she should be punished (Study 1) and the more he/she should be rewarded (Study 2). As such, these findings are the first to demonstrate that third-party sanctioning decisions are moderated by environmental uncertainty. 相似文献
35.
Laurie A. Rudman Corinne A. Moss-Racusin Julie E. Phelan Sanne Nauts 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2012,48(1):165-179
Agentic female leaders risk social and economic penalties for behaving counter-stereotypically (i.e., backlash; Rudman, 1998), but what motivates prejudice against female leaders? The status incongruity hypothesis (SIH) proposes that agentic women are penalized for status violations because doing so defends the gender hierarchy. Consistent with this view, Study 1 found that women are proscribed from dominant, high status displays (which are reserved for leaders and men); Studies 2–3 revealed that prejudice against agentic female leaders was mediated by a dominance penalty; and in Study 3, participants' gender system-justifying beliefs moderated backlash effects. Study 4 found that backlash was exacerbated when perceivers were primed with a system threat. Study 5 showed that only female leaders who threatened the status quo suffered sabotage. In concert, support for the SIH suggests that backlash functions to preserve male dominance by reinforcing a double standard for power and control. 相似文献
36.
Domicele Jonauskaite;Déborah Epicoco;Abdulrahman S. Al-rasheed;John Jamir Benzon R. Aruta;Victoria Bogushevskaya;Sanne G. Brederoo;Violeta Corona;Sergejs Fomins;Alena Gizdic;Yulia A. Griber;Jelena Havelka;Marco Hirnstein;George John;Daniela S. Jopp;Bodil Karlsson;Nikos Konstantinou;Éric Laurent;Lynn Marquardt;Philip C. Mefoh;Daniel Oberfeld;Marietta Papadatou-Pastou;Corinna M. Perchtold-Stefan;Giulia F. M. Spagnulo;Aygun Sultanova;Takumi Tanaka;Ma. Criselda Tengco-Pacquing;Mari Uusküla;Grażyna Wąsowicz;Christine Mohr; 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2024,115(2):275-305
As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour–emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour–emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour–emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour. 相似文献
37.
P. Matthijs Bal Edina Dóci Xander Lub Yvonne G. T. Van Rossenberg Sanne Nijs Safâa Achnak 《European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology》2019,28(3):289-299
This manifesto presents 10 recommendations for a sustainable future for the field of Work and Organizational Psychology. The manifesto is the result of an emerging movement around the Future of WOP (see www.futureofwop.com), which aims to bring together WOP-scholars committed to actively contribute to building a better future for our field. Our recommendations are intended to support both individuals and collectives to become actively engaged in co-creating the future of WOP together with us. Therefore, this manifesto is open and never “finished.” It should continuously evolve, based on an ongoing debate around our professional values and behavior. This manifesto is meant, first of all, for ourselves as an academic community. Furthermore, it is also important for managers, decision makers, and other stakeholders and interested parties, such as students, governments and organizations, as we envision what the future of WOP could look like, and it is only through our collective efforts that we will be able to realize a sustainable future for all of us. 相似文献
38.
Mohsen Joshanloo Marié P. Wissing Itumeleng P. Khumalo Sanne M.A. Lamers 《Personality and individual differences》2013
This study investigated the factorial structure and invariance of the Mental Health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF) across cultural groups from three nations, namely, the Netherlands, South Africa, and Iran (N = 1120). The three-dimensional structure of mental well-being was supported in all the groups. The results of measurement invariance testing confirmed the full metric and partial scalar invariance of the MHC-SF. The study also compared the latent means for the aspects of mental well-being in the three cultural groups, and found significant differences. The significance and implications of the results are discussed. 相似文献
39.
Moral ideals should not be seen as simple and purely personal, but as complex values with a social dimension that transcend attempts to formulate or realize them. Orientation towards ideals needs a realistic component: people should identify with the quest for an ideal, not with the ideal itself, and consider the possibility of negative consequences of their pursuit. Such realism about ideals includes acknowledging that ideals are not the only, nor the most important, motivating force of morality. 相似文献
40.
In this experimental study, we made an attempt to examine gender-related peer influences on childhood fear. Nine- to 12-year-old
boys and girls were provided with ambiguous and positive information about novel animals and then asked to provide a subjective
fear rating of the animals under two conditions: fear of one animal was assessed individually by the child on its own, whereas
fear of the other animal was measured after a brief discussion on fear-related issues with a same-gender peer. Results indicated
that children who completed the FBQ after a discussion with a same-gender peer displayed lower fear beliefs scores than children
who completed the questionnaire fully on their own. This fear-suppression effect was mainly evident in boys after hearing
ambiguous information about the novel animals. The implications of these findings are briefly discussed. 相似文献