首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   165篇
  免费   33篇
  国内免费   20篇
  218篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   9篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   16篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   14篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   2篇
  1998年   1篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
排序方式: 共有218条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
同伴交往、亲子交往与儿童心理理论发展的关系   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
采用经典的心理理论任务、同伴提名法、父母教养方式问卷以及在游戏情境中观察同伴交往行为和亲子交往行为等多种方法,探讨同伴交往、亲子交往与儿童心理理论发展的关系.88名3-6岁儿童及其家长参加了实验.研究结果表明:(1)交往类型中受欢迎儿童,其心理理论发展水平显著高于被拒绝和被忽视儿童.(2)民主型教养方式最有利于促进幼儿心理理论的发展,放任型教养方式最不利于幼儿心理理论的发展.(3)家长指导游戏的行为、分享情感的行为和使用心理状态术语的行为,有利于促进孩子心理理论的发展,而对孩子的冷漠最不利于孩子心理理论的发展.(4)相对于同伴交往,亲子交往对儿童心理理论的发展有更显著的预测力,其中家长和孩子的情感分享.对孩子遇到问题的解释及指导是促进孩子心理理论发展的重要影响因素.  相似文献   
132.
影响儿童情绪自我调节的因素   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
情绪自我调节较多的研究主要集中在有关情绪自我调节的表现方式和发展阶段水平方面,本文则从情绪自我调节的积极指向方面探讨其影响因素,认为大脑成熟顺序、社会认知发展及文化宗教等方面的因素影响着儿童的情绪自我调节.为对儿童的发展进行更好的干预提供依据.  相似文献   
133.
父母意识的结构与内涵初探   总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16  
桑标  唐剑 《心理科学》2000,23(3):279-284
本研究尝试性地提出了父母意识的概念,运用自行设计的母性意识、父性意识调查表对300对父母作评估与分析,来探讨父母意识的结构与内涵.因素分析结果表明,母性意识可归为13种主要因子,累积解释率为59.6%,其中最突出的是自我丧失感和亲子一体感;父性意识可归为9种主要因子,累积解释率为51.2%,最为突出的是对家庭的接纳感和责任感.父母意识的结构与内涵丰富而复杂,且父性意识与母性意识有所不同.  相似文献   
134.
李同吉  桑标 《心理科学》2005,28(1):128-129,124
本文介绍了行动观的主要理论观点,认为个体有意识的自我发展和自我调节对于发展具有重大意义,进而阐述了自我观察、自我评价和自我调节的过程和机制。行动观克服了哲学的决定论和因果机制的局限,把发展的多样性和规律性统一起来,并有效地整合了文化和个体自身对于发展的作用。  相似文献   
135.
Openness to Experience is an important but relatively poorly understood personality construct. Advances in openness research require further construct clarification as well as establishment of a common framework for conceptualizing and measuring the lower level structure of the construct. In this article, we present data from 3 studies to address this research need. In Study 1, we identify 6 facets of Openness to Experience—intellectual efficiency, ingenuity, curiosity, aesthetics, tolerance, and depthbased on a factor analysis of 36 existing Openness-related scales. In Study 2, we present further validity evidence for the 6-facet structure based on a newly developed measure of Openness. Data from this study also suggest the presence of 2 intermediate-level factors (i.e., aspects) of Openness: intellect and culture. In Study 3, we present a short form of the newly developed measure, retaining items that showed the highest internal consistency and measurement invariance across 3 samples: U.S. undergraduates, Chinese MBA students, and Chinese undergraduates. Together these 3 studies offer a more nuanced understanding of the multifaceted nature of the Openness construct.  相似文献   
136.
To illustrate the importance of facet-level investigations in predicting organizational outcomes and the need for more primary studies on this topic, we conducted a meta-analysis that took an exploratory look at differential relationships among 7 organizational criteria and Openness traits varying in breadth. Nine Openness predictors—the global dimension, 2 aspects (intellect and culture), and 6 facets (intellectual efficiency, ingenuity, curiosity, aesthetics, tolerance, and depth)—were examined in relations with various organizational criteria such as traditional performance outcomes (task performance, contextual performance, counterproductive work behavior), turnover, leadership effectiveness, training performance, and adaptive performance. Our results support the idea that Openness facets could exhibit differential validity for many organizational outcomes.  相似文献   
137.
138.
Perception of a facial expression can be altered or biased by a prolonged viewing of other facial expressions, known as the facial expression adaptation aftereffect (FEAA). Recent studies using antiexpressions have demonstrated a monotonic relation between the magnitude of the FEAA and adaptor extremity, suggesting that facial expressions are opponent coded and represented continuously from one expression to its antiexpression. However, it is unclear whether the opponent-coding scheme can account for the FEAA between two facial expressions. In the current study, we demonstrated that the magnitude of the FEAA between two facial expressions increased monotonically as a function of the intensity of adapting facial expressions, consistent with the predictions based on the opponent-coding model. Further, the monotonic increase in the FEAA occurred even when the intensity of an adapting face was too weak for its expression to be recognized. These results together suggest that multiple facial expressions are encoded and represented by balanced activity of neural populations tuned to different facial expressions.  相似文献   
139.
论影响儿童能力认识的内外部因素   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
吕凯  桑标 《心理科学》2008,31(2):463-466
儿童能力认识发展是元认知发展的重要方面,儿童对自身能力的认识与评价对于其成就动机、自我概念和人格特征等发展具有重要影响.儿童的能力认识受到多种因素的影响,而且这些因素之间存在复杂的交互作用,使得儿童能力认识呈现出复杂化特点.本文简要分析了影响儿童能力认识的内外部因素及其关系,并对该领域未来研究方向做一展望.  相似文献   
140.
The purposes of the present study were to identify latent profiles using dimensions of the Career Flexibility Inventory and to examine the mean differences of the latent profiles and related variables. Career flexibility was identified to have 5 latent profiles: passive wavering, mediocre, rigid thinking, moderately flexible, and productively flexible. The productively flexible profile reported the highest scores and the passive wavering profile reported the lowest scores of life satisfaction and self-determination. Each profile showed specific trend of mean differences with career indecision, intolerance of uncertainty, life satisfaction, self-determination, and the 2 dimensions of the Vocational Identity Status Assessment.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号