全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1293篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1300篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 33篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 47篇 |
2013年 | 125篇 |
2012年 | 59篇 |
2011年 | 45篇 |
2010年 | 46篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 52篇 |
2007年 | 54篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 44篇 |
2004年 | 41篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 36篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有1300条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
811.
King EC Lee TA McKay SM Scovil CY Peters AL Pratt J Maki BE 《Human movement science》2011,30(2):368-383
A fundamental principle that has emerged from studies of natural gaze behavior is that goal-directed arm movements are typically guided by a saccade to the target. In this study, we evaluated a hypothesis that this principle does not apply to rapid reach-to-grasp movements evoked by sudden unexpected balance perturbations. These perturbations involved forward translation of a large (2 × 6 m) motion platform configured to simulate a “real-life” environment. Subjects performed a common “daily-life” visuo-cognitive task (find a telephone and make a call) that required walking to the end of the platform, which was triggered to move as they approached a handrail mounted alongside the travel path. A deception was used to ensure that the perturbation was truly unexpected. Eleven of 18 healthy young-adult subjects (age 22-30) reached to grasp or touch the rail in response to the balance perturbation. In support of the hypothesis, none of these arm reactions was guided by concurrent visual fixation of the handrail. Seven of the 11 looked at the rail upon first entering the environment, and hence may have used “stored” central-field information about the handrail location to guide the subsequent arm reaction. However, the other four subjects never looked directly at the rail, indicating a complete reliance on peripheral vision. These findings add to previous evidence of distinctions in the CNS control of volitional and perturbation-evoked arm movements. Future studies will determine whether similar visuo-motor behavior occurs when the available handhold is smaller or when subjects are not engaged in a concurrent visuo-cognitive task. 相似文献
812.
Hale S Rose NS Myerson J Strube MJ Sommers M Tye-Murray N Spehar B 《Psychology and aging》2011,26(1):92-110
The present study addresses three questions regarding age differences in working memory: (1) whether performance on complex span tasks decreases as a function of age at a faster rate than performance on simple span tasks; (2) whether spatial working memory decreases at a faster rate than verbal working memory; and (3) whether the structure of working memory abilities is different for different age groups. Adults, ages 20-89 (n = 388), performed three simple and three complex verbal span tasks and three simple and three complex spatial memory tasks. Performance on the spatial tasks decreased at faster rates as a function of age than performance on the verbal tasks, but within each domain, performance on complex and simple span tasks decreased at the same rates. Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that domain-differentiated models yielded better fits than models involving domain-general constructs, providing further evidence of the need to distinguish verbal and spatial working memory abilities. Regardless of which domain-differentiated model was examined, and despite the faster rates of decrease in the spatial domain, age group comparisons revealed that the factor structure of working memory abilities was highly similar in younger and older adults and showed no evidence of age-related dedifferentiation. 相似文献
813.
Sandra Praxmarer 《Journal of Consumer Behaviour》2011,10(4):225-231
This paper analyzes the effects of message strength on brand attitude when consumers simulate product usage. The relevant literature suggests that advertising message strength does not matter when consumers imagine product usage, but previous studies do not consider the potential moderating effects of involvement and product category knowledge. The results of this research demonstrate that among high‐involvement consumers who perceive their knowledge in the product category as limited, imagining product usage does not reduce the relevance of a strong advertising message. This paper also shows that overall, imagining product usage positively affects brand attitude – regardless of message strength, involvement, and self‐perceived knowledge. An important practical implication arises from the study. Marketers should encourage mental simulation in their advertisements, but be careful not to set a good argument aside if the product/service is highly relevant to potential customers and the potential customers do not perceive themselves as very knowledgeable about the product category. This finding is particularly relevant to complex or relatively new products, because here, the group of consumers who are highly involved but do not perceive themselves as very knowledgeable may be large. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
814.
The present study examined the effect of worry versus relaxation and neutral thought activity on both physiological and subjective responding to positive and negative emotional stimuli. Thirty-eight participants with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and 35 nonanxious control participants were randomly assigned to engage in worry, relaxation, or neutral inductions prior to sequential exposure to each of four emotion-inducing film clips. The clips were designed to elicit fear, sadness, happiness, and calm emotions. Self reported negative and positive affect was assessed following each induction and exposure, and vagal activity was measured throughout. Results indicate that worry (vs. relaxation) led to reduced vagal tone for the GAD group, as well as higher negative affect levels for both groups. Additionally, prior worry resulted in less physiological and subjective responding to the fearful film clip, and reduced negative affect in response to the sad clip. This suggests that worry may facilitate avoidance of processing negative emotions by way of preventing a negative emotional contrast. Implications for the role of worry in emotion avoidance are discussed. 相似文献
815.
Transitive inference is a complex task, conducive to the use of multiple strategies. We investigated whether transitive inference
accuracy can be improved by biasing strategy choice towards a proposition-based approach that relies on the extraction of
relations among stimuli. We biased strategy choice by using familiar stimuli with known relations that tap prior knowledge.
Semantic information led to increased accuracy for younger and older adults, and increased awareness of stimulus relations.
Increased age was associated with reduced awareness. Awareness accounted for the variability in performance accuracy to a
greater extent than age, as aware older and younger adults showed similar accuracies on all conditions. The current work indicates
that age differences in performance can be minimized by providing semantically meaningful stimuli that bias participants to
use a relational proposition-based approach. 相似文献
816.
Jörg M. Müller Christian Postert Thomas Beyer Tilman Furniss Sandra Achtergarde 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(2):246-254
Eleven short versions of the Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) assessing general psychopathology, containing 5 to 53 items, were
compared on the basis of data from a sample of one hundred mothers of 0-to-6-year-old children referred for treatment at a
Child Psychiatric Family Day Hospital in Münster, Germany. The SCL short versions were compared with regard to internal consistency,
sensitivity and specificity, ability to distinguish between subjects by a new test index (PDTS), and association with indicators
of validity (SCL-90-R Global Severity Index, BDI scores). All short versions showed almost equally high internal consistency,
sensitivity and specificity, and high correlations with validity indices. The PDTS test index describes a ‘good’ ability of
the original SCL-90-R to differentiate between subjects, a ‘moderate’ performance for the BSI, the HSCL-25 and the SCL-27,
and a ‘poor’ performance of the very short forms—according to the standards of interpreting PDTS scores. The SCL-10S is recommended
for screening purposes because this scale represented the best compromise between economy and accuracy. However, for other
research and clinical purposes, the use of one of the longer short versions (BSI, HSCL-25, or SCL-27) is recommended because
of their superior discriminative ability. 相似文献
817.
Esterberg ML Goulding SM Walker EF 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(4):515-528
Cluster A personality disorders (PD), including schizotypal personality disorder (SPD), paranoid personality disorder (PPD),
and schizoid PD, are marked by odd and eccentric behaviors, and are grouped together because of common patterns in symptomatology
as well as shared genetic and environmental risk factors. The DSM-IV-TR describes personality disorders as representing stable
and enduring patterns of maladaptive traits, and much of what is understood about Cluster A personality disorders in particular
stems from research with adult populations. Less in known about these disorders in children and adolescents, and controversy
remains regarding diagnosis of personality disorders in general in youth. The current paper reviews the available research
on Cluster A personality disorders in childhood and adolescence; specifically, we discuss differentiating between the three
disorders and distinguishing them from other syndromes, measuring Cluster A disorders in youth, and the nature and course
of these disorders throughout childhood and adolescence. We also present recent longitudinal data from a sample of adolescents
diagnosed with Cluster A personality disorders from our research laboratory, and suggest directions for future research in
this important but understudied area. 相似文献
818.
Sandra L. Calvert Brian A. Mahler Sean M. Zehnder Abby Jenkins Mickey S. Lee 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》2003,24(6):627-644
Preadolescent children who did not know one another interacted in a multiuser domain (MUD), an online site designed to facilitate identity exploration and peer interaction. Each child participated in two separate sessions, one with a same-sex and one with an opposite-sex peer. Children created characters that reflected real-life properties of themselves, such as gender and interests in popular culture. Boys in same-sex pairs interacted with one another through action, rapid changes, and playful exchanges. Girls in same-sex pairs interacted primarily through written dialogue. In mixed pairs, boys wrote more and engaged in less playful exchanges, and girls wrote fewer and increased their actions. The results suggest that boys and girls have their own unique play styles with same-sex peers, but will moderate those patterns during late childhood to communicate with peers of the opposite sex. 相似文献
819.
This article describes a systematized method for training clinical staff from a variety of mental health professions while implementing supportive, process‐oriented multiple family therapy (MFT) in a children's psychiatric hospital. The model uses a step‐by‐step approach based upon learning objectives specific to the theory and practice of MFT for each stage of the process. External consultants skilled and experienced in MFT supported tuition in the initial stages. The method employs seminars with theoretical and experiential components, highlighting the major conceptual features of group, family therapy and MFT. Advanced stages of the training model include experiential learning in small groups and ongoing peer group supervision with live families. The systematic procedure has relevance for ongoing supervision, research and best standards of clinical practice in the hospital. 相似文献
820.