首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4662篇
  免费   243篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2023年   49篇
  2022年   49篇
  2021年   52篇
  2020年   118篇
  2019年   120篇
  2018年   168篇
  2017年   163篇
  2016年   171篇
  2015年   109篇
  2014年   146篇
  2013年   507篇
  2012年   214篇
  2011年   228篇
  2010年   169篇
  2009年   152篇
  2008年   188篇
  2007年   213篇
  2006年   191篇
  2005年   161篇
  2004年   146篇
  2003年   146篇
  2002年   137篇
  2001年   102篇
  2000年   64篇
  1999年   65篇
  1998年   52篇
  1997年   46篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   55篇
  1994年   62篇
  1993年   43篇
  1992年   58篇
  1991年   42篇
  1990年   39篇
  1989年   36篇
  1988年   34篇
  1987年   34篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   38篇
  1984年   38篇
  1983年   33篇
  1982年   32篇
  1981年   26篇
  1980年   28篇
  1979年   35篇
  1978年   43篇
  1977年   35篇
  1976年   22篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   27篇
排序方式: 共有4916条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Identification of CV syllables was studied in a backward masking paradigm in order to examine two types of interactions observed between dichotically presented speech sounds: the feature sharing effect and the lag effect. Pairs of syllables differed in the consonant, the vowel, and their relative times of onset. Interference between the two dichotic inputs was observed primarily for pairs which contrasted on voicing. Performance on pairs that shared voicing remained excellent under all three conditions. The results suggest that the interference underlying the lag effect and the feature sharing effect for voicing occur before phonetic analysis where both auditory inputs interact.  相似文献   
992.
Several programs have been written in the FOCAL, FORTRAN, and BASIC languages for reformatting and analyzing SKED data. These programs include selection and explicit labeling of sets of recording counters representing distributions and/or total counts of events, several general manipulations of distributional data, and standard statistical treatment of distributions.  相似文献   
993.
Human infants learning to stand use visual proprioceptive information about body sway in order to maintain stable posture. Moreover, the visual proprioceptive information is more potent than the nonvisual. This is shown by an experiment in which infants were caused to sway and even fall forward or backward in response to appropriate visual stimulation.  相似文献   
994.
Key pecking by pigeons was reinforced with food under second-order schedules with fixed-ratio units. A constant total number of key pecks was required for reinforcement under each condition, but the size and, inversely, number of fixed-ratio components were varied. The total response requirement of 256 pecks was divided into fixed-ratio units of 128, 64, 32, 8, and 2 responses. A brief stimulus, which always preceded food reinforcement, was presented upon completion of each fixed-ratio unit. Under most conditions, the pattern of within-unit responding was typical of that under simple fixed-ratio schedules. Overall response rate was an inverted U-shaped function of component size. That is, response rates were highest under moderate sized units (fixed ratio 128 and 64). This relationship is consistent with previous determinations of rate as a function of fixed-ratio value for simple fixed-ratio schedules.  相似文献   
995.
Binocular stereopsis has traditionally been studied mainly under static viewing conditions. There has consequently been the tendency to view binocular stereopsis only in terms of the pickup of purely spatial (time-frozen) disparity. However, whenever there is movement of objects or the 0, the structure of the light entering each eye undergoes continuous change, and so a different type of disparity—kinetic disparity—is made potentially available to the binocular system. That kinetic disparity can, in fact, be picked up is shown by the present experiment, in which there was no spatial disparity information available about the three-dimensional motion path of an object; only kinetic disparity information was available. This suggests that a clear distinction should be made between binocular-static and binocular-kinetic space perception.  相似文献   
996.
In a previous paper [Elashoff 1969], we derived optimal rater teams for a particular formulation of the dichotomous rater problem. Here, we describe a computer-based procedure for selecting good rater teams in practice; we apply the procedure to the selection of items for a psychological inventory. This research was supported in part by the author's predoctoral fellowship from the National Institutes of Health and by National Science Foundation Grant GS-341, and National Institutes of Health Grants FR-3 and FR-122.  相似文献   
997.
Generalizability of stratified-parallel tests   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
  相似文献   
998.
On each trial a sample point randomly drawn from one of two normal probability distributions was exhibited to S, who had to guess whether the sample was a “1” or a “2.” He was then given feedback, which was determined by which of the two distributions the sample point derived from. Two continua were employed: dot position on a file card, and groyness of square in. patches. Three levels of d’ were employed. The function giving the probability of a response “1” for different sample values was sharper for the higher d’ conditions, and for the dot position continuum. Cessation of feedback resulted in improved performance for the low d’ condition. Incentive payoff, confidence ratings, and experimenter had virtually no effects on task performance.  相似文献   
999.
In a previous paper (Dixon, 1958b) one of the authors reported an experiment which suggested that apparent changes in the threshold for one eye occur as a function of the emotionality of stimulus material presented below threshold to the other eye. The following experiment describes an attempt to investigate further the validity of this conclusion. The results are consistent with those from the previous research.  相似文献   
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号